Cognitive flexibility in children with Developmental Language Disorder: Drawing of nonexistent objects

Cognitive flexibility is the ability to adapt thoughts and behaviors to new environments. Previous studies investigating cognitive flexibility in children with Developmental Language Disorder (DLD) present contradictory findings. In the current study, cognitive flexibility was assessed in 5- and 6-y...

Full description

Bibliographic Details
Main Authors: Adi-Japha, E. (Author), Berke, R. (Author), Blom, E. (Author), Shaya, N. (Author)
Format: Article
Language:English
Published: Elsevier Inc. 2021
Subjects:
Online Access:View Fulltext in Publisher
LEADER 03356nam a2200613Ia 4500
001 10.1016-j.jcomdis.2021.106137
008 220427s2021 CNT 000 0 und d
020 |a 00219924 (ISSN) 
245 1 0 |a Cognitive flexibility in children with Developmental Language Disorder: Drawing of nonexistent objects 
260 0 |b Elsevier Inc.  |c 2021 
856 |z View Fulltext in Publisher  |u https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jcomdis.2021.106137 
520 3 |a Cognitive flexibility is the ability to adapt thoughts and behaviors to new environments. Previous studies investigating cognitive flexibility in children with Developmental Language Disorder (DLD) present contradictory findings. In the current study, cognitive flexibility was assessed in 5- and 6-year-old preschoolers with DLD (n = 23) and peers with typical development (TD; n = 50) using a nonexistent object drawing (NEOD) task. The children were asked to draw a nonexistent man and a nonexistent house. The children with DLD did not differ from their peers with TD on simple category changes, which were comprised of changes in the size or shape of parts of the object, change of the whole shape of the object, and deletion of parts of the object. Nevertheless, children with DLD made fewer more complex, high-level category changes, which included same-category insertions, position exchange of object's parts, and cross-category insertions. The difference between DLD and TD on high-level category changes was related to differences between the two groups in verbal short-term memory and inhibition. Furthermore, children with DLD made no changes to their original drawings of an existing man and house more often than their peers with TD. It is concluded that children with DLD aged 5–6 years show less flexibility on the NEOD task than age-matched children with TD. This difference in cognitive flexibility may be related to lower levels of verbal short-term memory and inhibition ability of children with DLD, or to different use of these cognitive skills on the NEOD task. © 2021 
650 0 4 |a Article 
650 0 4 |a child 
650 0 4 |a Child 
650 0 4 |a Child, Preschool 
650 0 4 |a clinical article 
650 0 4 |a cognition 
650 0 4 |a Cognition 
650 0 4 |a cognitive flexibility 
650 0 4 |a Cognitive flexibility 
650 0 4 |a controlled study 
650 0 4 |a developmental language disorder 
650 0 4 |a developmental language disorder 
650 0 4 |a drawing 
650 0 4 |a effect size 
650 0 4 |a female 
650 0 4 |a human 
650 0 4 |a Humans 
650 0 4 |a Inhibition 
650 0 4 |a inhibition (psychology) 
650 0 4 |a kindergarten 
650 0 4 |a language development 
650 0 4 |a Language Development Disorders 
650 0 4 |a Language disorders 
650 0 4 |a male 
650 0 4 |a Male 
650 0 4 |a Memory, Short-Term 
650 0 4 |a nonexistent object drawing 
650 0 4 |a Nonexistent object drawing 
650 0 4 |a peer group 
650 0 4 |a Peer Group 
650 0 4 |a preschool child 
650 0 4 |a short term memory 
650 0 4 |a size 
650 0 4 |a skill 
650 0 4 |a task performance 
650 0 4 |a verbal memory 
650 0 4 |a Verbal short-term memory 
700 1 |a Adi-Japha, E.  |e author 
700 1 |a Berke, R.  |e author 
700 1 |a Blom, E.  |e author 
700 1 |a Shaya, N.  |e author 
773 |t Journal of Communication Disorders