The color-coded cloze procedure : a method to assist adult ESL students in searching for clues to fill in cloze blanks
Research by Shanahan and Kamil (1982, 1983, 1984) suggested that students did not use inter-sentential information to improve their doze test scores. Chihara and Oiler (1977) had discovered in their research with more proficient ESL students the contrary. Building on the idea that the issue of...
Main Author: | |
---|---|
Language: | English |
Published: |
2008
|
Online Access: | http://hdl.handle.net/2429/3070 |
id |
ndltd-LACETR-oai-collectionscanada.gc.ca-BVAU.2429-3070 |
---|---|
record_format |
oai_dc |
collection |
NDLTD |
language |
English |
sources |
NDLTD |
description |
Research by Shanahan and Kamil (1982, 1983, 1984)
suggested that students did not use inter-sentential
information to improve their doze test scores. Chihara and
Oiler (1977) had discovered in their research with more
proficient ESL students the contrary. Building on the idea
that the issue of too-local reading depended on both the
proficiency and motivation of the students along with the
availability of beyond—sentence clues, the present research
project was designed using color-coded blanks and parts of
speech to highlight inter-sentential clues, especially re
iterative—word clues.
The purpose of the color—coding was to see if, by
differentiating blanks and words, students could focus more
sharply on the necessary information, primarily re—iterative
words that occurred beyond the sentence of each doze blank,
and use these words to fill the doze blanks. The students
in the treatment group were given a key to the relationship
between the color and the parts of speech but were not
explicitly told to look for re-iterative-word clues. It was
expected, however, that each colored blank would stimulate
the students to look through the passage to find a word of
the same color to fill the said blank. It was also the goal
to maximize the number of inter—sentential re—iterative
clues and see how cloze scores would be affected when color— coding was used as opposed to no color—coding (the major
control).
If the inter—sentential clues were present, then color—coding should have resulted in the treatment group having
better scores. Randomly—colored words were used as another
control to determine whether any positive effect had come
from the coding or just from color as a motivational factor.
Lastly, the pre- and post—tests were non—colored in order to
see if the color—coding treatment had just a temporary means
of help which led to no lasting gains once removed..
A pilot project was done with a class of twenty adult
upper—intermediate ESL students using four forms of a
standard doze test. Based on the results the colored—blank
form was dropped so the three classes in the main project
could each have one form. To suit the needs of the more
advanced university students, new stories were chosen and
prepared using a rationalized doze to maximize the number
of re-iterative—word clues. In the main research project
most of the random-color group (the least proficient group)
dropped out after the pre—test. The non—colored rational
doze group received higher scores than the color—coded
treatment group on all of the tests. When the mean scores
were graphed both these groups made steady progress from
practice test to practice test, the treatment group
appearing to almost catch up. Improvement was made from
pre- to post—test by both groups but less by the treatment group, especially when only inter—sentential blanks were
counted. Generally speaking, t—tests and a very sensitive
statistical program (“One Between and One Repeated Measures
Factor ANOVA”) confirmed this improvement but showed that
the color—coded doze treatment group and the non-colored
doze control group in most cases did not differ
significantly.
The graphical analyses of the results were more
optimistic in favor of the color-coded treatment than the
statistical analyses were but the small sample size (N = 13)
made the statistical findings unclear at times. Improvement
in some cases may have been because of the declining
readability levels of subsequent passages.
The number of blanks filled, the number filled
correctly, and the relationship of these two were analyzed
to determine the confidence and productive confidence levels
of each group. Results showed the color—coded treatment
group were less confident in filling blanks and made limited
gains in productive confidence over the control group. |
author |
Labrum, Howat Alan |
spellingShingle |
Labrum, Howat Alan The color-coded cloze procedure : a method to assist adult ESL students in searching for clues to fill in cloze blanks |
author_facet |
Labrum, Howat Alan |
author_sort |
Labrum, Howat Alan |
title |
The color-coded cloze procedure : a method to assist adult ESL students in searching for clues to fill in cloze blanks |
title_short |
The color-coded cloze procedure : a method to assist adult ESL students in searching for clues to fill in cloze blanks |
title_full |
The color-coded cloze procedure : a method to assist adult ESL students in searching for clues to fill in cloze blanks |
title_fullStr |
The color-coded cloze procedure : a method to assist adult ESL students in searching for clues to fill in cloze blanks |
title_full_unstemmed |
The color-coded cloze procedure : a method to assist adult ESL students in searching for clues to fill in cloze blanks |
title_sort |
color-coded cloze procedure : a method to assist adult esl students in searching for clues to fill in cloze blanks |
publishDate |
2008 |
url |
http://hdl.handle.net/2429/3070 |
work_keys_str_mv |
AT labrumhowatalan thecolorcodedclozeprocedureamethodtoassistadulteslstudentsinsearchingforcluestofillinclozeblanks AT labrumhowatalan colorcodedclozeprocedureamethodtoassistadulteslstudentsinsearchingforcluestofillinclozeblanks |
_version_ |
1716649984961544192 |
spelling |
ndltd-LACETR-oai-collectionscanada.gc.ca-BVAU.2429-30702014-03-14T15:38:38Z The color-coded cloze procedure : a method to assist adult ESL students in searching for clues to fill in cloze blanks Labrum, Howat Alan Research by Shanahan and Kamil (1982, 1983, 1984) suggested that students did not use inter-sentential information to improve their doze test scores. Chihara and Oiler (1977) had discovered in their research with more proficient ESL students the contrary. Building on the idea that the issue of too-local reading depended on both the proficiency and motivation of the students along with the availability of beyond—sentence clues, the present research project was designed using color-coded blanks and parts of speech to highlight inter-sentential clues, especially re iterative—word clues. The purpose of the color—coding was to see if, by differentiating blanks and words, students could focus more sharply on the necessary information, primarily re—iterative words that occurred beyond the sentence of each doze blank, and use these words to fill the doze blanks. The students in the treatment group were given a key to the relationship between the color and the parts of speech but were not explicitly told to look for re-iterative-word clues. It was expected, however, that each colored blank would stimulate the students to look through the passage to find a word of the same color to fill the said blank. It was also the goal to maximize the number of inter—sentential re—iterative clues and see how cloze scores would be affected when color— coding was used as opposed to no color—coding (the major control). If the inter—sentential clues were present, then color—coding should have resulted in the treatment group having better scores. Randomly—colored words were used as another control to determine whether any positive effect had come from the coding or just from color as a motivational factor. Lastly, the pre- and post—tests were non—colored in order to see if the color—coding treatment had just a temporary means of help which led to no lasting gains once removed.. A pilot project was done with a class of twenty adult upper—intermediate ESL students using four forms of a standard doze test. Based on the results the colored—blank form was dropped so the three classes in the main project could each have one form. To suit the needs of the more advanced university students, new stories were chosen and prepared using a rationalized doze to maximize the number of re-iterative—word clues. In the main research project most of the random-color group (the least proficient group) dropped out after the pre—test. The non—colored rational doze group received higher scores than the color—coded treatment group on all of the tests. When the mean scores were graphed both these groups made steady progress from practice test to practice test, the treatment group appearing to almost catch up. Improvement was made from pre- to post—test by both groups but less by the treatment group, especially when only inter—sentential blanks were counted. Generally speaking, t—tests and a very sensitive statistical program (“One Between and One Repeated Measures Factor ANOVA”) confirmed this improvement but showed that the color—coded doze treatment group and the non-colored doze control group in most cases did not differ significantly. The graphical analyses of the results were more optimistic in favor of the color-coded treatment than the statistical analyses were but the small sample size (N = 13) made the statistical findings unclear at times. Improvement in some cases may have been because of the declining readability levels of subsequent passages. The number of blanks filled, the number filled correctly, and the relationship of these two were analyzed to determine the confidence and productive confidence levels of each group. Results showed the color—coded treatment group were less confident in filling blanks and made limited gains in productive confidence over the control group. 2008-12-17T23:45:11Z 2008-12-17T23:45:11Z 1992 2008-12-17T23:45:11Z 1992-11 Electronic Thesis or Dissertation http://hdl.handle.net/2429/3070 eng UBC Retrospective Theses Digitization Project [http://www.library.ubc.ca/archives/retro_theses/] |