A robust dissociation among the language, multiple demand, and default mode networks: Evidence from inter-region correlations in effect size

Complex cognitive processes, including language, rely on multiple mental operations that are carried out by several large-scale functional networks in the frontal, temporal, and parietal association cortices of the human brain. The central division of cognitive labor is between two fronto-parietal b...

Full description

Bibliographic Details
Main Authors: Blank, I.A (Author), Fedorenko, E. (Author), Mahowald, K. (Author), Mineroff, Z. (Author)
Format: Article
Language:English
Published: Elsevier Ltd 2018
Subjects:
Online Access:View Fulltext in Publisher
LEADER 05206nam a2200817Ia 4500
001 10.1016-j.neuropsychologia.2018.09.011
008 220706s2018 CNT 000 0 und d
020 |a 00283932 (ISSN) 
245 1 0 |a A robust dissociation among the language, multiple demand, and default mode networks: Evidence from inter-region correlations in effect size 
260 0 |b Elsevier Ltd  |c 2018 
856 |z View Fulltext in Publisher  |u https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2018.09.011 
520 3 |a Complex cognitive processes, including language, rely on multiple mental operations that are carried out by several large-scale functional networks in the frontal, temporal, and parietal association cortices of the human brain. The central division of cognitive labor is between two fronto-parietal bilateral networks: (a) the multiple demand (MD) network, which supports executive processes, such as working memory and cognitive control, and is engaged by diverse task domains, including language, especially when comprehension gets difficult; and (b) the default mode network (DMN), which supports introspective processes, such as mind wandering, and is active when we are not engaged in processing external stimuli. These two networks are strongly dissociated in both their functional profiles and their patterns of activity fluctuations during naturalistic cognition. Here, we focus on the functional relationship between these two networks and a third network: (c) the fronto-temporal left-lateralized “core” language network, which is selectively recruited by linguistic processing. Is the language network distinct and dissociated from both the MD network and the DMN, or is it synchronized and integrated with one or both of them? Recent work has provided evidence for a dissociation between the language network and the MD network. However, the relationship between the language network and the DMN is less clear, with some evidence for coordinated activity patterns and similar response profiles, perhaps due to the role of both in semantic processing. Here we use a novel fMRI approach to examine the relationship among the three networks: we measure the strength of activations in different language, MD, and DMN regions to functional contrasts typically used to identify each network, and then test which regions co-vary in their contrast effect sizes across 60 individuals. We find that effect sizes correlate strongly within each network (e.g., one language region and another language region, or one DMN region and another DMN region), but show little or no correlation for region pairs across networks (e.g., a language region and a DMN region). Thus, using our novel method, we replicate the language/MD network dissociation discovered previously with other approaches, and also show that the language network is robustly dissociated from the DMN, overall suggesting that these three networks contribute to high-level cognition in different ways and, perhaps, support distinct computations. Inter-individual differences in effect sizes therefore do not simply reflect general differences in vascularization or attention, but exhibit sensitivity to the functional architecture of the brain. The strength of activation in each network can thus be probed separately in studies that attempt to link neural variability to behavioral or genetic variability. © 2018 Elsevier Ltd 
650 0 4 |a adult 
650 0 4 |a Adult 
650 0 4 |a article 
650 0 4 |a attention 
650 0 4 |a Attention 
650 0 4 |a brain 
650 0 4 |a Brain 
650 0 4 |a brain mapping 
650 0 4 |a Brain Mapping 
650 0 4 |a comprehension 
650 0 4 |a Comprehension 
650 0 4 |a controlled study 
650 0 4 |a default mode network 
650 0 4 |a Default mode network 
650 0 4 |a diagnostic imaging 
650 0 4 |a dissociation 
650 0 4 |a effect size 
650 0 4 |a executive function 
650 0 4 |a Executive Function 
650 0 4 |a female 
650 0 4 |a Female 
650 0 4 |a Functional Laterality 
650 0 4 |a functional magnetic resonance imaging 
650 0 4 |a Functional MRI 
650 0 4 |a genetic variability 
650 0 4 |a hemispheric dominance 
650 0 4 |a human 
650 0 4 |a human experiment 
650 0 4 |a Humans 
650 0 4 |a individuality 
650 0 4 |a Individuality 
650 0 4 |a Individual-subject analyses 
650 0 4 |a language 
650 0 4 |a Language 
650 0 4 |a Magnetic Resonance Imaging 
650 0 4 |a major clinical study 
650 0 4 |a male 
650 0 4 |a Male 
650 0 4 |a Memory, Short-Term 
650 0 4 |a middle aged 
650 0 4 |a Middle Aged 
650 0 4 |a Multiple demand network 
650 0 4 |a nerve tract 
650 0 4 |a Neural Pathways 
650 0 4 |a nuclear magnetic resonance imaging 
650 0 4 |a physiology 
650 0 4 |a self control 
650 0 4 |a Self-Control 
650 0 4 |a short term memory 
650 0 4 |a spatial memory 
650 0 4 |a Spatial Memory 
650 0 4 |a vascularization 
650 0 4 |a young adult 
650 0 4 |a Young Adult 
700 1 |a Blank, I.A.  |e author 
700 1 |a Fedorenko, E.  |e author 
700 1 |a Mahowald, K.  |e author 
700 1 |a Mineroff, Z.  |e author 
773 |t Neuropsychologia