|
|
|
|
LEADER |
03494nam a2200565Ia 4500 |
001 |
10.1016-j.cognition.2021.104628 |
008 |
220427s2021 CNT 000 0 und d |
020 |
|
|
|a 00100277 (ISSN)
|
245 |
1 |
0 |
|a The ontogeny of early language discrimination: Beyond rhythm
|
260 |
|
0 |
|b Elsevier B.V.
|c 2021
|
856 |
|
|
|z View Fulltext in Publisher
|u https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2021.104628
|
520 |
3 |
|
|a Infants can discriminate languages that belong to different rhythmic classes at birth. The ability to perform within-class discrimination emerges around the fifth month of life. The cues that infants use to discriminate between prosodically close languages remain elusive. Segmental information could be a potential cue, since infants notice vowel mispronunciations of their names, show the first signs of word recognition and the first signs of perceptual narrowing for vowels around 6 months of age. If infants have in place some proto-segmental information, most likely it is about vowels. Another potential cue infants may use to discriminate languages is intonation. We tested participants using sentences in Eastern Catalan, Western Catalan and Spanish. The two Catalan dialects and Spanish belong to the same rhythmic class, they are syllable-timed, but they differ in terms of vowel distribution, given that only Eastern Catalan has vocalic reduction. The vowel distributions of Western Catalan and Spanish are more comparable. However, they differ in terms of their intonational patterns. In Experiment 1, we tested the ability of 4.5-month-old infants learning Eastern Catalan and/or Spanish to discriminate between sentences in Eastern and Western Catalan and in Experiment 2 their ability to discriminate between sentences in Western Catalan and Spanish. In order to disentangle the contribution of segmental and suprasegmental information, we also tested infants using low-pass filtered sentences in the two dialects (Experiment 3) and low-pass filtered sentences in Western Catalan and Spanish (Experiment 4). Infants discriminated the two Catalan dialects only when the stimuli were natural sentences, whereas they were able to discriminate between Western Catalan and Spanish when the stimuli were either natural or low-pass filtered sentences. The research also provides evidence of equivalent language discrimination abilities in infants growing up in monolingual and bilingual environments. © 2021 Elsevier B.V.
|
650 |
0 |
4 |
|a article
|
650 |
0 |
4 |
|a bilingualism
|
650 |
0 |
4 |
|a Bilingualism
|
650 |
0 |
4 |
|a controlled study
|
650 |
0 |
4 |
|a female
|
650 |
0 |
4 |
|a human
|
650 |
0 |
4 |
|a human experiment
|
650 |
0 |
4 |
|a Humans
|
650 |
0 |
4 |
|a infant
|
650 |
0 |
4 |
|a Infant
|
650 |
0 |
4 |
|a Infant, Newborn
|
650 |
0 |
4 |
|a Infants
|
650 |
0 |
4 |
|a Intonation
|
650 |
0 |
4 |
|a language
|
650 |
0 |
4 |
|a Language
|
650 |
0 |
4 |
|a language development
|
650 |
0 |
4 |
|a Language Development
|
650 |
0 |
4 |
|a Language discrimination
|
650 |
0 |
4 |
|a learning
|
650 |
0 |
4 |
|a Learning
|
650 |
0 |
4 |
|a male
|
650 |
0 |
4 |
|a multilingualism
|
650 |
0 |
4 |
|a Multilingualism
|
650 |
0 |
4 |
|a Names
|
650 |
0 |
4 |
|a newborn
|
650 |
0 |
4 |
|a nomenclature
|
650 |
0 |
4 |
|a ontogeny
|
650 |
0 |
4 |
|a phonetics
|
650 |
0 |
4 |
|a Phonetics
|
650 |
0 |
4 |
|a rhythm
|
650 |
0 |
4 |
|a speech perception
|
650 |
0 |
4 |
|a Speech Perception
|
650 |
0 |
4 |
|a vowel
|
650 |
0 |
4 |
|a Vowel distribution
|
650 |
0 |
4 |
|a word recognition
|
700 |
1 |
|
|a Sebastian-Galles, N.
|e author
|
700 |
1 |
|
|a Zacharaki, K.
|e author
|
773 |
|
|
|t Cognition
|