|
|
|
|
LEADER |
03286nam a2200733Ia 4500 |
001 |
10.1016-j.cognition.2018.06.017 |
008 |
220706s2018 CNT 000 0 und d |
020 |
|
|
|a 00100277 (ISSN)
|
245 |
1 |
0 |
|a The relationship between parental mental-state language and 2.5-year-olds’ performance on a nontraditional false-belief task
|
260 |
|
0 |
|b Elsevier B.V.
|c 2018
|
856 |
|
|
|z View Fulltext in Publisher
|u https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2018.06.017
|
520 |
3 |
|
|a A growing body of evidence suggests that children succeed in nontraditional false-belief tasks in the first years of life. However, few studies have examined individual differences in infants’ and toddlers’ performance on these tasks. Here we investigated whether parental use of mental-state language (i.e. think, understand), which predicts children's performance on elicited-response false-belief tasks at older ages, also predicts toddlers’ performance on a nontraditional task. We tested 2.5-year-old children in a verbal nontraditional false-belief task that included two looking time measures, anticipatory looking and preferential looking, and measured parents’ use of mental-state language during a picture-book task. Parents’ use of mental-state language positively predicted children's performance on the anticipatory-looking measure of the nontraditional task. These results provide the first evidence that social factors relate to children's false-belief understanding prior to age 3 and that this association extends to performance on nontraditional tasks. These findings add to a growing number of studies suggesting that mental-state language supports mental-state understanding across the lifespan. © 2018 Elsevier B.V.
|
650 |
0 |
4 |
|a age
|
650 |
0 |
4 |
|a anticipation
|
650 |
0 |
4 |
|a Anticipation, Psychological
|
650 |
0 |
4 |
|a Article
|
650 |
0 |
4 |
|a child
|
650 |
0 |
4 |
|a child parent relation
|
650 |
0 |
4 |
|a Child, Preschool
|
650 |
0 |
4 |
|a comprehension
|
650 |
0 |
4 |
|a Comprehension
|
650 |
0 |
4 |
|a concept formation
|
650 |
0 |
4 |
|a Concept Formation
|
650 |
0 |
4 |
|a controlled study
|
650 |
0 |
4 |
|a cultural anthropology
|
650 |
0 |
4 |
|a Culture
|
650 |
0 |
4 |
|a False-belief understanding
|
650 |
0 |
4 |
|a father
|
650 |
0 |
4 |
|a female
|
650 |
0 |
4 |
|a Female
|
650 |
0 |
4 |
|a human
|
650 |
0 |
4 |
|a human experiment
|
650 |
0 |
4 |
|a Humans
|
650 |
0 |
4 |
|a language
|
650 |
0 |
4 |
|a language development
|
650 |
0 |
4 |
|a Language Development
|
650 |
0 |
4 |
|a male
|
650 |
0 |
4 |
|a Male
|
650 |
0 |
4 |
|a mental state language
|
650 |
0 |
4 |
|a mental task
|
650 |
0 |
4 |
|a Mental-state language
|
650 |
0 |
4 |
|a mother
|
650 |
0 |
4 |
|a parent
|
650 |
0 |
4 |
|a Parent-Child Relations
|
650 |
0 |
4 |
|a Parents
|
650 |
0 |
4 |
|a Photic Stimulation
|
650 |
0 |
4 |
|a photostimulation
|
650 |
0 |
4 |
|a physiology
|
650 |
0 |
4 |
|a preschool child
|
650 |
0 |
4 |
|a priority journal
|
650 |
0 |
4 |
|a procedures
|
650 |
0 |
4 |
|a Psychological reasoning
|
650 |
0 |
4 |
|a psychology
|
650 |
0 |
4 |
|a psychomotor performance
|
650 |
0 |
4 |
|a Psychomotor Performance
|
650 |
0 |
4 |
|a Random Allocation
|
650 |
0 |
4 |
|a randomization
|
650 |
0 |
4 |
|a social cognition
|
650 |
0 |
4 |
|a Social cognition
|
650 |
0 |
4 |
|a task performance
|
650 |
0 |
4 |
|a vocabulary
|
700 |
1 |
|
|a Roby, E.
|e author
|
700 |
1 |
|
|a Scott, R.M.
|e author
|
773 |
|
|
|t Cognition
|