The relationship between parental mental-state language and 2.5-year-olds’ performance on a nontraditional false-belief task

A growing body of evidence suggests that children succeed in nontraditional false-belief tasks in the first years of life. However, few studies have examined individual differences in infants’ and toddlers’ performance on these tasks. Here we investigated whether parental use of mental-state languag...

Full description

Bibliographic Details
Main Authors: Roby, E. (Author), Scott, R.M (Author)
Format: Article
Language:English
Published: Elsevier B.V. 2018
Subjects:
age
Online Access:View Fulltext in Publisher
LEADER 03286nam a2200733Ia 4500
001 10.1016-j.cognition.2018.06.017
008 220706s2018 CNT 000 0 und d
020 |a 00100277 (ISSN) 
245 1 0 |a The relationship between parental mental-state language and 2.5-year-olds’ performance on a nontraditional false-belief task 
260 0 |b Elsevier B.V.  |c 2018 
856 |z View Fulltext in Publisher  |u https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2018.06.017 
520 3 |a A growing body of evidence suggests that children succeed in nontraditional false-belief tasks in the first years of life. However, few studies have examined individual differences in infants’ and toddlers’ performance on these tasks. Here we investigated whether parental use of mental-state language (i.e. think, understand), which predicts children's performance on elicited-response false-belief tasks at older ages, also predicts toddlers’ performance on a nontraditional task. We tested 2.5-year-old children in a verbal nontraditional false-belief task that included two looking time measures, anticipatory looking and preferential looking, and measured parents’ use of mental-state language during a picture-book task. Parents’ use of mental-state language positively predicted children's performance on the anticipatory-looking measure of the nontraditional task. These results provide the first evidence that social factors relate to children's false-belief understanding prior to age 3 and that this association extends to performance on nontraditional tasks. These findings add to a growing number of studies suggesting that mental-state language supports mental-state understanding across the lifespan. © 2018 Elsevier B.V. 
650 0 4 |a age 
650 0 4 |a anticipation 
650 0 4 |a Anticipation, Psychological 
650 0 4 |a Article 
650 0 4 |a child 
650 0 4 |a child parent relation 
650 0 4 |a Child, Preschool 
650 0 4 |a comprehension 
650 0 4 |a Comprehension 
650 0 4 |a concept formation 
650 0 4 |a Concept Formation 
650 0 4 |a controlled study 
650 0 4 |a cultural anthropology 
650 0 4 |a Culture 
650 0 4 |a False-belief understanding 
650 0 4 |a father 
650 0 4 |a female 
650 0 4 |a Female 
650 0 4 |a human 
650 0 4 |a human experiment 
650 0 4 |a Humans 
650 0 4 |a language 
650 0 4 |a language development 
650 0 4 |a Language Development 
650 0 4 |a male 
650 0 4 |a Male 
650 0 4 |a mental state language 
650 0 4 |a mental task 
650 0 4 |a Mental-state language 
650 0 4 |a mother 
650 0 4 |a parent 
650 0 4 |a Parent-Child Relations 
650 0 4 |a Parents 
650 0 4 |a Photic Stimulation 
650 0 4 |a photostimulation 
650 0 4 |a physiology 
650 0 4 |a preschool child 
650 0 4 |a priority journal 
650 0 4 |a procedures 
650 0 4 |a Psychological reasoning 
650 0 4 |a psychology 
650 0 4 |a psychomotor performance 
650 0 4 |a Psychomotor Performance 
650 0 4 |a Random Allocation 
650 0 4 |a randomization 
650 0 4 |a social cognition 
650 0 4 |a Social cognition 
650 0 4 |a task performance 
650 0 4 |a vocabulary 
700 1 |a Roby, E.  |e author 
700 1 |a Scott, R.M.  |e author 
773 |t Cognition