Watching others in a positive state does not induce optimism bias in common marmosets (Callithrix jacchus), but leads to behaviour indicative of competition

Emotional contagion is suggested to facilitate group life by enhancing synchronized responses to the environment. Cooperative breeders are an example of a social system that requires such intricate coordination between individuals. Therefore, we studied emotional contagion in common marmosets by mea...

Full description

Bibliographic Details
Main Authors: Adriaense, J.E.C (Author), Bugnyar, T. (Author), Hintze, S. (Author), Lamm, C. (Author), Marshall, L. (Author), Šlipogor, V. (Author)
Format: Article
Language:English
Published: Springer Science and Business Media Deutschland GmbH 2021
Subjects:
Online Access:View Fulltext in Publisher
LEADER 03097nam a2200397Ia 4500
001 10.1007-s10071-021-01497-1
008 220427s2021 CNT 000 0 und d
020 |a 14359448 (ISSN) 
245 1 0 |a Watching others in a positive state does not induce optimism bias in common marmosets (Callithrix jacchus), but leads to behaviour indicative of competition 
260 0 |b Springer Science and Business Media Deutschland GmbH  |c 2021 
856 |z View Fulltext in Publisher  |u https://doi.org/10.1007/s10071-021-01497-1 
520 3 |a Emotional contagion is suggested to facilitate group life by enhancing synchronized responses to the environment. Cooperative breeders are an example of a social system that requires such intricate coordination between individuals. Therefore, we studied emotional contagion in common marmosets by means of a judgement bias test. Demonstrators were exposed to an emotion manipulation (i.e., positive, negative, control), and observers perceived only the demonstrator’s behaviour. We predicted that the positive or negative states of the demonstrator would induce matching states in the observer, indicating emotional contagion. All subjects’ emotional states were assessed through behaviour and cognition, the latter by means of a judgement bias test. Behavioural results showed a successful emotion manipulation of demonstrators, with manipulation-congruent expressions (i.e., positive calls in the positive condition, and negative calls and pilo-erect tail in the negative condition). Observers showed no manipulation-congruent expressions, but showed more scratching and arousal after the positive manipulation. Concerning the judgement bias test, we predicted that subjects in a positive state should increase their response to ambiguous cues (i.e., optimism bias), and subjects in a negative state should decrease their response (i.e., pessimism bias). This prediction was not supported as neither demonstrators nor observers showed such bias in either manipulation. Yet, demonstrators showed an increased response to the near-positive cue, and additional analyses showed unexpected responses to the reference cues, as well as a researcher identity effect. We discuss all results combined, including recently raised validation concerns of the judgement bias test, and inherent challenges to empirically studying emotional contagion. © 2021, The Author(s). 
650 0 4 |a animal 
650 0 4 |a Animal emotions 
650 0 4 |a Animals 
650 0 4 |a association 
650 0 4 |a Callithrix 
650 0 4 |a Callithrix 
650 0 4 |a cognition 
650 0 4 |a Cognition 
650 0 4 |a Cognitive bias 
650 0 4 |a Cues 
650 0 4 |a decision making 
650 0 4 |a emotion 
650 0 4 |a Emotional contagion 
650 0 4 |a Emotions 
650 0 4 |a Judgement bias test 
650 0 4 |a Judgment 
650 0 4 |a Social cognition 
700 1 |a Adriaense, J.E.C.  |e author 
700 1 |a Bugnyar, T.  |e author 
700 1 |a Hintze, S.  |e author 
700 1 |a Lamm, C.  |e author 
700 1 |a Marshall, L.  |e author 
700 1 |a Šlipogor, V.  |e author 
773 |t Animal Cognition