Kinematics of visually-guided eye movements.

One of the hallmarks of an eye movement that follows Listing's law is the half-angle rule that says that the angular velocity of the eye tilts by half the angle of eccentricity of the line of sight relative to primary eye position. Since all visually-guided eye movements in the regime of far vi...

Full description

Bibliographic Details
Main Authors: Bernhard J M Hess, Jakob S Thomassen
Format: Article
Language:English
Published: Public Library of Science (PLoS) 2014-01-01
Series:PLoS ONE
Online Access:http://europepmc.org/articles/PMC3994052?pdf=render
id doaj-d70877d9ffa94deca3ded45fb87e6818
record_format Article
spelling doaj-d70877d9ffa94deca3ded45fb87e68182020-11-25T00:12:40ZengPublic Library of Science (PLoS)PLoS ONE1932-62032014-01-0194e9523410.1371/journal.pone.0095234Kinematics of visually-guided eye movements.Bernhard J M HessJakob S ThomassenOne of the hallmarks of an eye movement that follows Listing's law is the half-angle rule that says that the angular velocity of the eye tilts by half the angle of eccentricity of the line of sight relative to primary eye position. Since all visually-guided eye movements in the regime of far viewing follow Listing's law (with the head still and upright), the question about its origin is of considerable importance. Here, we provide theoretical and experimental evidence that Listing's law results from a unique motor strategy that allows minimizing ocular torsion while smoothly tracking objects of interest along any path in visual space. The strategy consists in compounding conventional ocular rotations in meridian planes, that is in horizontal, vertical and oblique directions (which are all torsion-free) with small linear displacements of the eye in the frontal plane. Such compound rotation-displacements of the eye can explain the kinematic paradox that the fixation point may rotate in one plane while the eye rotates in other planes. Its unique signature is the half-angle law in the position domain, which means that the rotation plane of the eye tilts by half-the angle of gaze eccentricity. We show that this law does not readily generalize to the velocity domain of visually-guided eye movements because the angular eye velocity is the sum of two terms, one associated with rotations in meridian planes and one associated with displacements of the eye in the frontal plane. While the first term does not depend on eye position the second term does depend on eye position. We show that compounded rotation - displacements perfectly predict the average smooth kinematics of the eye during steady- state pursuit in both the position and velocity domain.http://europepmc.org/articles/PMC3994052?pdf=render
collection DOAJ
language English
format Article
sources DOAJ
author Bernhard J M Hess
Jakob S Thomassen
spellingShingle Bernhard J M Hess
Jakob S Thomassen
Kinematics of visually-guided eye movements.
PLoS ONE
author_facet Bernhard J M Hess
Jakob S Thomassen
author_sort Bernhard J M Hess
title Kinematics of visually-guided eye movements.
title_short Kinematics of visually-guided eye movements.
title_full Kinematics of visually-guided eye movements.
title_fullStr Kinematics of visually-guided eye movements.
title_full_unstemmed Kinematics of visually-guided eye movements.
title_sort kinematics of visually-guided eye movements.
publisher Public Library of Science (PLoS)
series PLoS ONE
issn 1932-6203
publishDate 2014-01-01
description One of the hallmarks of an eye movement that follows Listing's law is the half-angle rule that says that the angular velocity of the eye tilts by half the angle of eccentricity of the line of sight relative to primary eye position. Since all visually-guided eye movements in the regime of far viewing follow Listing's law (with the head still and upright), the question about its origin is of considerable importance. Here, we provide theoretical and experimental evidence that Listing's law results from a unique motor strategy that allows minimizing ocular torsion while smoothly tracking objects of interest along any path in visual space. The strategy consists in compounding conventional ocular rotations in meridian planes, that is in horizontal, vertical and oblique directions (which are all torsion-free) with small linear displacements of the eye in the frontal plane. Such compound rotation-displacements of the eye can explain the kinematic paradox that the fixation point may rotate in one plane while the eye rotates in other planes. Its unique signature is the half-angle law in the position domain, which means that the rotation plane of the eye tilts by half-the angle of gaze eccentricity. We show that this law does not readily generalize to the velocity domain of visually-guided eye movements because the angular eye velocity is the sum of two terms, one associated with rotations in meridian planes and one associated with displacements of the eye in the frontal plane. While the first term does not depend on eye position the second term does depend on eye position. We show that compounded rotation - displacements perfectly predict the average smooth kinematics of the eye during steady- state pursuit in both the position and velocity domain.
url http://europepmc.org/articles/PMC3994052?pdf=render
work_keys_str_mv AT bernhardjmhess kinematicsofvisuallyguidedeyemovements
AT jakobsthomassen kinematicsofvisuallyguidedeyemovements
_version_ 1725398220629082112