The size-weight illusion comes along with improved weight discrimination.

When people judge the weight of two objects of equal mass but different size, they perceive the smaller one as being heavier. Up to date, there is no consensus about the mechanisms which give rise to this size-weight illusion. We recently suggested a model that describes heaviness perception as a we...

Full description

Bibliographic Details
Main Authors: Christian Wolf, Knut Drewing
Format: Article
Language:English
Published: Public Library of Science (PLoS) 2020-01-01
Series:PLoS ONE
Online Access:https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0236440
id doaj-b47124d2e9cb4627a79516d9850dd56c
record_format Article
spelling doaj-b47124d2e9cb4627a79516d9850dd56c2021-03-03T21:59:25ZengPublic Library of Science (PLoS)PLoS ONE1932-62032020-01-01157e023644010.1371/journal.pone.0236440The size-weight illusion comes along with improved weight discrimination.Christian WolfKnut DrewingWhen people judge the weight of two objects of equal mass but different size, they perceive the smaller one as being heavier. Up to date, there is no consensus about the mechanisms which give rise to this size-weight illusion. We recently suggested a model that describes heaviness perception as a weighted average of two sensory heaviness estimates with correlated noise: one estimate derived from mass, the other one derived from density. The density estimate is first derived from mass and size, but at the final perceptual level, perceived heaviness is biased by an object's density, not by its size. Here, we tested the models' prediction that weight discrimination of equal-size objects is better in lifting conditions which are prone to the size-weight illusion as compared to conditions lacking (the essentially uninformative) size information. This is predicted because in these objects density covaries with mass, and according to the model density serves as an additional sensory cue. Participants performed a two-interval forced-choice weight discrimination task. We manipulated the quality of either haptic (Experiment 1) or visual (Experiment 2) size information and measured just-noticeable differences (JNDs). Both for the haptic and the visual illusion, JNDs were lower in lifting conditions in which size information was available. Thus, when heaviness perception can be influenced by an object's density, it is more reliable. This discrimination benefit under conditions that provide the additional information that objects are of equal size is further support for the role of density and the integration of sensory estimates in the size-weight illusion.https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0236440
collection DOAJ
language English
format Article
sources DOAJ
author Christian Wolf
Knut Drewing
spellingShingle Christian Wolf
Knut Drewing
The size-weight illusion comes along with improved weight discrimination.
PLoS ONE
author_facet Christian Wolf
Knut Drewing
author_sort Christian Wolf
title The size-weight illusion comes along with improved weight discrimination.
title_short The size-weight illusion comes along with improved weight discrimination.
title_full The size-weight illusion comes along with improved weight discrimination.
title_fullStr The size-weight illusion comes along with improved weight discrimination.
title_full_unstemmed The size-weight illusion comes along with improved weight discrimination.
title_sort size-weight illusion comes along with improved weight discrimination.
publisher Public Library of Science (PLoS)
series PLoS ONE
issn 1932-6203
publishDate 2020-01-01
description When people judge the weight of two objects of equal mass but different size, they perceive the smaller one as being heavier. Up to date, there is no consensus about the mechanisms which give rise to this size-weight illusion. We recently suggested a model that describes heaviness perception as a weighted average of two sensory heaviness estimates with correlated noise: one estimate derived from mass, the other one derived from density. The density estimate is first derived from mass and size, but at the final perceptual level, perceived heaviness is biased by an object's density, not by its size. Here, we tested the models' prediction that weight discrimination of equal-size objects is better in lifting conditions which are prone to the size-weight illusion as compared to conditions lacking (the essentially uninformative) size information. This is predicted because in these objects density covaries with mass, and according to the model density serves as an additional sensory cue. Participants performed a two-interval forced-choice weight discrimination task. We manipulated the quality of either haptic (Experiment 1) or visual (Experiment 2) size information and measured just-noticeable differences (JNDs). Both for the haptic and the visual illusion, JNDs were lower in lifting conditions in which size information was available. Thus, when heaviness perception can be influenced by an object's density, it is more reliable. This discrimination benefit under conditions that provide the additional information that objects are of equal size is further support for the role of density and the integration of sensory estimates in the size-weight illusion.
url https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0236440
work_keys_str_mv AT christianwolf thesizeweightillusioncomesalongwithimprovedweightdiscrimination
AT knutdrewing thesizeweightillusioncomesalongwithimprovedweightdiscrimination
AT christianwolf sizeweightillusioncomesalongwithimprovedweightdiscrimination
AT knutdrewing sizeweightillusioncomesalongwithimprovedweightdiscrimination
_version_ 1714813979129282560