The geometric preference subtype in ASD: identifying a consistent, early-emerging phenomenon through eye tracking

Abstract Background The wide range of ability and disability in ASD creates a need for tools that parse the phenotypic heterogeneity into meaningful subtypes. Using eye tracking, our past studies revealed that when presented with social and geometric images, a subset of ASD toddlers preferred viewin...

Full description

Bibliographic Details
Main Authors: Adrienne Moore, Madeline Wozniak, Andrew Yousef, Cindy Carter Barnes, Debra Cha, Eric Courchesne, Karen Pierce
Format: Article
Language:English
Published: BMC 2018-03-01
Series:Molecular Autism
Subjects:
Online Access:http://link.springer.com/article/10.1186/s13229-018-0202-z
id doaj-a80baf9a61604c12a5f69f9afb465c9c
record_format Article
spelling doaj-a80baf9a61604c12a5f69f9afb465c9c2020-11-24T21:00:33ZengBMCMolecular Autism2040-23922018-03-019111310.1186/s13229-018-0202-zThe geometric preference subtype in ASD: identifying a consistent, early-emerging phenomenon through eye trackingAdrienne Moore0Madeline Wozniak1Andrew Yousef2Cindy Carter Barnes3Debra Cha4Eric Courchesne5Karen Pierce6Autism Center of Excellence, Department of Neurosciences, University of California San DiegoAutism Center of Excellence, Department of Neurosciences, University of California San DiegoAutism Center of Excellence, Department of Neurosciences, University of California San DiegoAutism Center of Excellence, Department of Neurosciences, University of California San DiegoAutism Center of Excellence, Department of Neurosciences, University of California San DiegoAutism Center of Excellence, Department of Neurosciences, University of California San DiegoAutism Center of Excellence, Department of Neurosciences, University of California San DiegoAbstract Background The wide range of ability and disability in ASD creates a need for tools that parse the phenotypic heterogeneity into meaningful subtypes. Using eye tracking, our past studies revealed that when presented with social and geometric images, a subset of ASD toddlers preferred viewing geometric images, and these toddlers also had greater symptom severity than ASD toddlers with greater social attention. This study tests whether this “GeoPref test” effect would generalize across different social stimuli. Methods Two hundred and twenty-seven toddlers (76 ASD) watched a 90-s video, the Complex Social GeoPref test, of dynamic geometric images paired with social images of children interacting and moving. Proportion of visual fixation time and number of saccades per second to both images were calculated. To allow for cross-paradigm comparisons, a subset of 126 toddlers also participated in the original GeoPref test. Measures of cognitive and social functioning (MSEL, ADOS, VABS) were collected and related to eye tracking data. To examine utility as a diagnostic indicator to detect ASD toddlers, validation statistics (e.g., sensitivity, specificity, ROC, AUC) were calculated for the Complex Social GeoPref test alone and when combined with the original GeoPref test. Results ASD toddlers spent a significantly greater amount of time viewing geometric images than any other diagnostic group. Fixation patterns from ASD toddlers who participated in both tests revealed a significant correlation, supporting the idea that these tests identify a phenotypically meaningful ASD subgroup. Combined use of both original and Complex Social GeoPref tests identified a subgroup of about 1 in 3 ASD toddlers from the “GeoPref” subtype (sensitivity 35%, specificity 94%, AUC 0.75.) Replicating our previous studies, more time looking at geometric images was associated with significantly greater ADOS symptom severity. Conclusions Regardless of the complexity of the social images used (low in the original GeoPref test vs high in the new Complex Social GeoPref test), eye tracking of toddlers can accurately identify a specific ASD “GeoPref” subtype with elevated symptom severity. The GeoPref tests are predictive of ASD at the individual subject level and thus potentially useful for various clinical applications (e.g., early identification, prognosis, or development of subtype-specific treatments).http://link.springer.com/article/10.1186/s13229-018-0202-zEye trackingAutism spectrum disorderEarly identificationSocial attentionGeometric preference
collection DOAJ
language English
format Article
sources DOAJ
author Adrienne Moore
Madeline Wozniak
Andrew Yousef
Cindy Carter Barnes
Debra Cha
Eric Courchesne
Karen Pierce
spellingShingle Adrienne Moore
Madeline Wozniak
Andrew Yousef
Cindy Carter Barnes
Debra Cha
Eric Courchesne
Karen Pierce
The geometric preference subtype in ASD: identifying a consistent, early-emerging phenomenon through eye tracking
Molecular Autism
Eye tracking
Autism spectrum disorder
Early identification
Social attention
Geometric preference
author_facet Adrienne Moore
Madeline Wozniak
Andrew Yousef
Cindy Carter Barnes
Debra Cha
Eric Courchesne
Karen Pierce
author_sort Adrienne Moore
title The geometric preference subtype in ASD: identifying a consistent, early-emerging phenomenon through eye tracking
title_short The geometric preference subtype in ASD: identifying a consistent, early-emerging phenomenon through eye tracking
title_full The geometric preference subtype in ASD: identifying a consistent, early-emerging phenomenon through eye tracking
title_fullStr The geometric preference subtype in ASD: identifying a consistent, early-emerging phenomenon through eye tracking
title_full_unstemmed The geometric preference subtype in ASD: identifying a consistent, early-emerging phenomenon through eye tracking
title_sort geometric preference subtype in asd: identifying a consistent, early-emerging phenomenon through eye tracking
publisher BMC
series Molecular Autism
issn 2040-2392
publishDate 2018-03-01
description Abstract Background The wide range of ability and disability in ASD creates a need for tools that parse the phenotypic heterogeneity into meaningful subtypes. Using eye tracking, our past studies revealed that when presented with social and geometric images, a subset of ASD toddlers preferred viewing geometric images, and these toddlers also had greater symptom severity than ASD toddlers with greater social attention. This study tests whether this “GeoPref test” effect would generalize across different social stimuli. Methods Two hundred and twenty-seven toddlers (76 ASD) watched a 90-s video, the Complex Social GeoPref test, of dynamic geometric images paired with social images of children interacting and moving. Proportion of visual fixation time and number of saccades per second to both images were calculated. To allow for cross-paradigm comparisons, a subset of 126 toddlers also participated in the original GeoPref test. Measures of cognitive and social functioning (MSEL, ADOS, VABS) were collected and related to eye tracking data. To examine utility as a diagnostic indicator to detect ASD toddlers, validation statistics (e.g., sensitivity, specificity, ROC, AUC) were calculated for the Complex Social GeoPref test alone and when combined with the original GeoPref test. Results ASD toddlers spent a significantly greater amount of time viewing geometric images than any other diagnostic group. Fixation patterns from ASD toddlers who participated in both tests revealed a significant correlation, supporting the idea that these tests identify a phenotypically meaningful ASD subgroup. Combined use of both original and Complex Social GeoPref tests identified a subgroup of about 1 in 3 ASD toddlers from the “GeoPref” subtype (sensitivity 35%, specificity 94%, AUC 0.75.) Replicating our previous studies, more time looking at geometric images was associated with significantly greater ADOS symptom severity. Conclusions Regardless of the complexity of the social images used (low in the original GeoPref test vs high in the new Complex Social GeoPref test), eye tracking of toddlers can accurately identify a specific ASD “GeoPref” subtype with elevated symptom severity. The GeoPref tests are predictive of ASD at the individual subject level and thus potentially useful for various clinical applications (e.g., early identification, prognosis, or development of subtype-specific treatments).
topic Eye tracking
Autism spectrum disorder
Early identification
Social attention
Geometric preference
url http://link.springer.com/article/10.1186/s13229-018-0202-z
work_keys_str_mv AT adriennemoore thegeometricpreferencesubtypeinasdidentifyingaconsistentearlyemergingphenomenonthrougheyetracking
AT madelinewozniak thegeometricpreferencesubtypeinasdidentifyingaconsistentearlyemergingphenomenonthrougheyetracking
AT andrewyousef thegeometricpreferencesubtypeinasdidentifyingaconsistentearlyemergingphenomenonthrougheyetracking
AT cindycarterbarnes thegeometricpreferencesubtypeinasdidentifyingaconsistentearlyemergingphenomenonthrougheyetracking
AT debracha thegeometricpreferencesubtypeinasdidentifyingaconsistentearlyemergingphenomenonthrougheyetracking
AT ericcourchesne thegeometricpreferencesubtypeinasdidentifyingaconsistentearlyemergingphenomenonthrougheyetracking
AT karenpierce thegeometricpreferencesubtypeinasdidentifyingaconsistentearlyemergingphenomenonthrougheyetracking
AT adriennemoore geometricpreferencesubtypeinasdidentifyingaconsistentearlyemergingphenomenonthrougheyetracking
AT madelinewozniak geometricpreferencesubtypeinasdidentifyingaconsistentearlyemergingphenomenonthrougheyetracking
AT andrewyousef geometricpreferencesubtypeinasdidentifyingaconsistentearlyemergingphenomenonthrougheyetracking
AT cindycarterbarnes geometricpreferencesubtypeinasdidentifyingaconsistentearlyemergingphenomenonthrougheyetracking
AT debracha geometricpreferencesubtypeinasdidentifyingaconsistentearlyemergingphenomenonthrougheyetracking
AT ericcourchesne geometricpreferencesubtypeinasdidentifyingaconsistentearlyemergingphenomenonthrougheyetracking
AT karenpierce geometricpreferencesubtypeinasdidentifyingaconsistentearlyemergingphenomenonthrougheyetracking
_version_ 1716779398687883264