Frontal White Matter Hyperintensities and Executive Functioning Performance in Older Adults

Frontal lobe structures decline faster than most other brain regions in older adults. Age-related change in the frontal lobe is associated with poorer executive function (e.g., working memory, switching/set-shifting, and inhibitory control). The effects and presence of frontal lobe white matter hype...

Full description

Bibliographic Details
Main Authors: Emanuel M. Boutzoukas, Andrew O'Shea, Alejandro Albizu, Nicole D. Evangelista, Hanna K. Hausman, Jessica N. Kraft, Emily J. Van Etten, Pradyumna K. Bharadwaj, Samantha G. Smith, Hyun Song, Eric C. Porges, Alex Hishaw, Steven T. DeKosky, Samuel S. Wu, Michael Marsiske, Gene E. Alexander, Ronald Cohen, Adam J. Woods
Format: Article
Language:English
Published: Frontiers Media S.A. 2021-06-01
Series:Frontiers in Aging Neuroscience
Subjects:
Online Access:https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fnagi.2021.672535/full
id doaj-93a863182b9141759e27564035bc76ed
record_format Article
collection DOAJ
language English
format Article
sources DOAJ
author Emanuel M. Boutzoukas
Emanuel M. Boutzoukas
Andrew O'Shea
Alejandro Albizu
Alejandro Albizu
Nicole D. Evangelista
Nicole D. Evangelista
Hanna K. Hausman
Hanna K. Hausman
Jessica N. Kraft
Jessica N. Kraft
Emily J. Van Etten
Pradyumna K. Bharadwaj
Samantha G. Smith
Hyun Song
Eric C. Porges
Eric C. Porges
Alex Hishaw
Alex Hishaw
Steven T. DeKosky
Samuel S. Wu
Michael Marsiske
Michael Marsiske
Gene E. Alexander
Gene E. Alexander
Ronald Cohen
Ronald Cohen
Adam J. Woods
Adam J. Woods
Adam J. Woods
spellingShingle Emanuel M. Boutzoukas
Emanuel M. Boutzoukas
Andrew O'Shea
Alejandro Albizu
Alejandro Albizu
Nicole D. Evangelista
Nicole D. Evangelista
Hanna K. Hausman
Hanna K. Hausman
Jessica N. Kraft
Jessica N. Kraft
Emily J. Van Etten
Pradyumna K. Bharadwaj
Samantha G. Smith
Hyun Song
Eric C. Porges
Eric C. Porges
Alex Hishaw
Alex Hishaw
Steven T. DeKosky
Samuel S. Wu
Michael Marsiske
Michael Marsiske
Gene E. Alexander
Gene E. Alexander
Ronald Cohen
Ronald Cohen
Adam J. Woods
Adam J. Woods
Adam J. Woods
Frontal White Matter Hyperintensities and Executive Functioning Performance in Older Adults
Frontiers in Aging Neuroscience
cognitive aging
executive function
NIH toolbox
white matter hyperintensities
region-specific hyperintensities
frontal lobes
author_facet Emanuel M. Boutzoukas
Emanuel M. Boutzoukas
Andrew O'Shea
Alejandro Albizu
Alejandro Albizu
Nicole D. Evangelista
Nicole D. Evangelista
Hanna K. Hausman
Hanna K. Hausman
Jessica N. Kraft
Jessica N. Kraft
Emily J. Van Etten
Pradyumna K. Bharadwaj
Samantha G. Smith
Hyun Song
Eric C. Porges
Eric C. Porges
Alex Hishaw
Alex Hishaw
Steven T. DeKosky
Samuel S. Wu
Michael Marsiske
Michael Marsiske
Gene E. Alexander
Gene E. Alexander
Ronald Cohen
Ronald Cohen
Adam J. Woods
Adam J. Woods
Adam J. Woods
author_sort Emanuel M. Boutzoukas
title Frontal White Matter Hyperintensities and Executive Functioning Performance in Older Adults
title_short Frontal White Matter Hyperintensities and Executive Functioning Performance in Older Adults
title_full Frontal White Matter Hyperintensities and Executive Functioning Performance in Older Adults
title_fullStr Frontal White Matter Hyperintensities and Executive Functioning Performance in Older Adults
title_full_unstemmed Frontal White Matter Hyperintensities and Executive Functioning Performance in Older Adults
title_sort frontal white matter hyperintensities and executive functioning performance in older adults
publisher Frontiers Media S.A.
series Frontiers in Aging Neuroscience
issn 1663-4365
publishDate 2021-06-01
description Frontal lobe structures decline faster than most other brain regions in older adults. Age-related change in the frontal lobe is associated with poorer executive function (e.g., working memory, switching/set-shifting, and inhibitory control). The effects and presence of frontal lobe white matter hyperintensities (WMH) on executive function in normal aging is relatively unknown. The current study assessed relationships between region-specific frontal WMH load and cognitive performance in healthy older adults using three executive function tasks from the NIH Toolbox (NIHTB) Cognition Battery. A cohort of 279 healthy older adults ages 65–88 completed NIHTB and 3T T1-weighted and FLAIR MRI. Lesion Segmentation Toolbox quantified WMH volume and generated lesion probability maps. Individual lesion maps were registered to the Desikan-Killiany atlas in FreeSurfer 6.0 to define regions of interest (ROI). Independent linear regressions assessed relationships between executive function performance and region-specific WMH in frontal lobe ROIs. All models included age, sex, education, estimated total intracranial volume, multi-site scanner differences, and cardiovascular disease risk using Framingham criteria as covariates. Poorer set-shifting performance was associated with greater WMH load in three frontal ROIs including bilateral superior frontal (left β = −0.18, FDR-p = 0.02; right β = −0.20, FDR-p = 0.01) and right medial orbitofrontal (β = −0.17, FDR-p = 0.02). Poorer inhibitory performance associated with higher WMH load in one frontal ROI, the right superior frontal (right β = −0.21, FDR-p = 0.01). There were no significant associations between working memory and WMH in frontal ROIs. Our study demonstrates that location and pattern of frontal WMH may be important to assess when examining age-related differences in cognitive functions involving switching/set-shifting and inhibition. On the other hand, working memory performance was not related to presence of frontal WMH in this sample. These data suggest that WMH may contribute selectively to age-related declines in executive function. Findings emerged beyond predictors known to be associated with WMH presence, including age and cardiovascular disease risk. The spread of WMH within the frontal lobes may play a key role in the neuropsychological profile of cognitive aging. Further research should explore whether early intervention on modifiable vascular factors or cognitive interventions targeted for executive abilities may help mitigate the effect of frontal WMH on executive function.
topic cognitive aging
executive function
NIH toolbox
white matter hyperintensities
region-specific hyperintensities
frontal lobes
url https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fnagi.2021.672535/full
work_keys_str_mv AT emanuelmboutzoukas frontalwhitematterhyperintensitiesandexecutivefunctioningperformanceinolderadults
AT emanuelmboutzoukas frontalwhitematterhyperintensitiesandexecutivefunctioningperformanceinolderadults
AT andrewoshea frontalwhitematterhyperintensitiesandexecutivefunctioningperformanceinolderadults
AT alejandroalbizu frontalwhitematterhyperintensitiesandexecutivefunctioningperformanceinolderadults
AT alejandroalbizu frontalwhitematterhyperintensitiesandexecutivefunctioningperformanceinolderadults
AT nicoledevangelista frontalwhitematterhyperintensitiesandexecutivefunctioningperformanceinolderadults
AT nicoledevangelista frontalwhitematterhyperintensitiesandexecutivefunctioningperformanceinolderadults
AT hannakhausman frontalwhitematterhyperintensitiesandexecutivefunctioningperformanceinolderadults
AT hannakhausman frontalwhitematterhyperintensitiesandexecutivefunctioningperformanceinolderadults
AT jessicankraft frontalwhitematterhyperintensitiesandexecutivefunctioningperformanceinolderadults
AT jessicankraft frontalwhitematterhyperintensitiesandexecutivefunctioningperformanceinolderadults
AT emilyjvanetten frontalwhitematterhyperintensitiesandexecutivefunctioningperformanceinolderadults
AT pradyumnakbharadwaj frontalwhitematterhyperintensitiesandexecutivefunctioningperformanceinolderadults
AT samanthagsmith frontalwhitematterhyperintensitiesandexecutivefunctioningperformanceinolderadults
AT hyunsong frontalwhitematterhyperintensitiesandexecutivefunctioningperformanceinolderadults
AT ericcporges frontalwhitematterhyperintensitiesandexecutivefunctioningperformanceinolderadults
AT ericcporges frontalwhitematterhyperintensitiesandexecutivefunctioningperformanceinolderadults
AT alexhishaw frontalwhitematterhyperintensitiesandexecutivefunctioningperformanceinolderadults
AT alexhishaw frontalwhitematterhyperintensitiesandexecutivefunctioningperformanceinolderadults
AT steventdekosky frontalwhitematterhyperintensitiesandexecutivefunctioningperformanceinolderadults
AT samuelswu frontalwhitematterhyperintensitiesandexecutivefunctioningperformanceinolderadults
AT michaelmarsiske frontalwhitematterhyperintensitiesandexecutivefunctioningperformanceinolderadults
AT michaelmarsiske frontalwhitematterhyperintensitiesandexecutivefunctioningperformanceinolderadults
AT geneealexander frontalwhitematterhyperintensitiesandexecutivefunctioningperformanceinolderadults
AT geneealexander frontalwhitematterhyperintensitiesandexecutivefunctioningperformanceinolderadults
AT ronaldcohen frontalwhitematterhyperintensitiesandexecutivefunctioningperformanceinolderadults
AT ronaldcohen frontalwhitematterhyperintensitiesandexecutivefunctioningperformanceinolderadults
AT adamjwoods frontalwhitematterhyperintensitiesandexecutivefunctioningperformanceinolderadults
AT adamjwoods frontalwhitematterhyperintensitiesandexecutivefunctioningperformanceinolderadults
AT adamjwoods frontalwhitematterhyperintensitiesandexecutivefunctioningperformanceinolderadults
_version_ 1721356777271853056
spelling doaj-93a863182b9141759e27564035bc76ed2021-06-28T07:48:31ZengFrontiers Media S.A.Frontiers in Aging Neuroscience1663-43652021-06-011310.3389/fnagi.2021.672535672535Frontal White Matter Hyperintensities and Executive Functioning Performance in Older AdultsEmanuel M. Boutzoukas0Emanuel M. Boutzoukas1Andrew O'Shea2Alejandro Albizu3Alejandro Albizu4Nicole D. Evangelista5Nicole D. Evangelista6Hanna K. Hausman7Hanna K. Hausman8Jessica N. Kraft9Jessica N. Kraft10Emily J. Van Etten11Pradyumna K. Bharadwaj12Samantha G. Smith13Hyun Song14Eric C. Porges15Eric C. Porges16Alex Hishaw17Alex Hishaw18Steven T. DeKosky19Samuel S. Wu20Michael Marsiske21Michael Marsiske22Gene E. Alexander23Gene E. Alexander24Ronald Cohen25Ronald Cohen26Adam J. Woods27Adam J. Woods28Adam J. Woods29Center for Cognitive Aging and Memory, McKnight Brain Institute, University of Florida, Gainesville, FL, United StatesDepartment of Clinical and Health Psychology, College of Public Health and Health Professions, University of Florida, Gainesville, FL, United StatesCenter for Cognitive Aging and Memory, McKnight Brain Institute, University of Florida, Gainesville, FL, United StatesCenter for Cognitive Aging and Memory, McKnight Brain Institute, University of Florida, Gainesville, FL, United StatesDepartment of Neuroscience, College of Medicine, University of Florida, Gainesville, FL, United StatesCenter for Cognitive Aging and Memory, McKnight Brain Institute, University of Florida, Gainesville, FL, United StatesDepartment of Clinical and Health Psychology, College of Public Health and Health Professions, University of Florida, Gainesville, FL, United StatesCenter for Cognitive Aging and Memory, McKnight Brain Institute, University of Florida, Gainesville, FL, United StatesDepartment of Clinical and Health Psychology, College of Public Health and Health Professions, University of Florida, Gainesville, FL, United StatesCenter for Cognitive Aging and Memory, McKnight Brain Institute, University of Florida, Gainesville, FL, United StatesDepartment of Neuroscience, College of Medicine, University of Florida, Gainesville, FL, United StatesDepartment of Psychology and Evelyn F. McKnight Brain Institute, University of Arizona, Tucson, AZ, United StatesDepartment of Psychology and Evelyn F. McKnight Brain Institute, University of Arizona, Tucson, AZ, United StatesDepartment of Psychology and Evelyn F. McKnight Brain Institute, University of Arizona, Tucson, AZ, United StatesDepartment of Psychology and Evelyn F. McKnight Brain Institute, University of Arizona, Tucson, AZ, United StatesCenter for Cognitive Aging and Memory, McKnight Brain Institute, University of Florida, Gainesville, FL, United StatesDepartment of Clinical and Health Psychology, College of Public Health and Health Professions, University of Florida, Gainesville, FL, United StatesDepartment Psychiatry, College of Medicine, University of Arizona, Tucson, AZ, United StatesDepartment of Neurology, College of Medicine, University of Arizona, Tucson, AZ, United StatesDepartment of Neurology, College of Medicine, University of Florida, Gainesville, FL, United StatesDepartment of Biostatistics, College of Public Health and Health Professions, College of Medicine, University of Florida, Gainesville, FL, United StatesCenter for Cognitive Aging and Memory, McKnight Brain Institute, University of Florida, Gainesville, FL, United StatesDepartment of Clinical and Health Psychology, College of Public Health and Health Professions, University of Florida, Gainesville, FL, United StatesDepartment of Psychology and Evelyn F. McKnight Brain Institute, University of Arizona, Tucson, AZ, United StatesDepartment of Psychiatry, Neuroscience and Physiological Sciences Graduate Interdisciplinary Programs, and BIO5 Institute, University of Arizona and Arizona Alzheimer's Disease Consortium, Tucson, AZ, United StatesCenter for Cognitive Aging and Memory, McKnight Brain Institute, University of Florida, Gainesville, FL, United StatesDepartment of Clinical and Health Psychology, College of Public Health and Health Professions, University of Florida, Gainesville, FL, United StatesCenter for Cognitive Aging and Memory, McKnight Brain Institute, University of Florida, Gainesville, FL, United StatesDepartment of Clinical and Health Psychology, College of Public Health and Health Professions, University of Florida, Gainesville, FL, United StatesDepartment of Neuroscience, College of Medicine, University of Florida, Gainesville, FL, United StatesFrontal lobe structures decline faster than most other brain regions in older adults. Age-related change in the frontal lobe is associated with poorer executive function (e.g., working memory, switching/set-shifting, and inhibitory control). The effects and presence of frontal lobe white matter hyperintensities (WMH) on executive function in normal aging is relatively unknown. The current study assessed relationships between region-specific frontal WMH load and cognitive performance in healthy older adults using three executive function tasks from the NIH Toolbox (NIHTB) Cognition Battery. A cohort of 279 healthy older adults ages 65–88 completed NIHTB and 3T T1-weighted and FLAIR MRI. Lesion Segmentation Toolbox quantified WMH volume and generated lesion probability maps. Individual lesion maps were registered to the Desikan-Killiany atlas in FreeSurfer 6.0 to define regions of interest (ROI). Independent linear regressions assessed relationships between executive function performance and region-specific WMH in frontal lobe ROIs. All models included age, sex, education, estimated total intracranial volume, multi-site scanner differences, and cardiovascular disease risk using Framingham criteria as covariates. Poorer set-shifting performance was associated with greater WMH load in three frontal ROIs including bilateral superior frontal (left β = −0.18, FDR-p = 0.02; right β = −0.20, FDR-p = 0.01) and right medial orbitofrontal (β = −0.17, FDR-p = 0.02). Poorer inhibitory performance associated with higher WMH load in one frontal ROI, the right superior frontal (right β = −0.21, FDR-p = 0.01). There were no significant associations between working memory and WMH in frontal ROIs. Our study demonstrates that location and pattern of frontal WMH may be important to assess when examining age-related differences in cognitive functions involving switching/set-shifting and inhibition. On the other hand, working memory performance was not related to presence of frontal WMH in this sample. These data suggest that WMH may contribute selectively to age-related declines in executive function. Findings emerged beyond predictors known to be associated with WMH presence, including age and cardiovascular disease risk. The spread of WMH within the frontal lobes may play a key role in the neuropsychological profile of cognitive aging. Further research should explore whether early intervention on modifiable vascular factors or cognitive interventions targeted for executive abilities may help mitigate the effect of frontal WMH on executive function.https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fnagi.2021.672535/fullcognitive agingexecutive functionNIH toolboxwhite matter hyperintensitiesregion-specific hyperintensitiesfrontal lobes