The processing of the Dutch masculine generic zijn 'his' across stereotype contexts: An eye-tracking study.

Language users often infer a person's gender when it is not explicitly mentioned. This information is included in the mental model of the described situation, giving rise to expectations regarding the continuation of the discourse. Such gender inferences can be based on two types of information...

Full description

Bibliographic Details
Main Authors: Theresa Redl, Anita Eerland, Ted J M Sanders
Format: Article
Language:English
Published: Public Library of Science (PLoS) 2018-01-01
Series:PLoS ONE
Online Access:http://europepmc.org/articles/PMC6193704?pdf=render
id doaj-8affee9dd03d435a9e56527a78effab4
record_format Article
spelling doaj-8affee9dd03d435a9e56527a78effab42020-11-25T01:52:52ZengPublic Library of Science (PLoS)PLoS ONE1932-62032018-01-011310e020590310.1371/journal.pone.0205903The processing of the Dutch masculine generic zijn 'his' across stereotype contexts: An eye-tracking study.Theresa RedlAnita EerlandTed J M SandersLanguage users often infer a person's gender when it is not explicitly mentioned. This information is included in the mental model of the described situation, giving rise to expectations regarding the continuation of the discourse. Such gender inferences can be based on two types of information: gender stereotypes (e.g., nurses are female) and masculine generics, which are grammatically masculine word forms that are used to refer to all genders in certain contexts (e.g., To each his own). In this eye-tracking experiment (N = 82), which is the first to systematically investigate the online processing of masculine generic pronouns, we tested whether the frequently used Dutch masculine generic zijn 'his' leads to a male bias. In addition, we tested the effect of context by introducing male, female, and neutral stereotypes. We found no evidence for the hypothesis that the generically-intended masculine pronoun zijn 'his' results in a male bias. However, we found an effect of stereotype context. After introducing a female stereotype, reading about a man led to an increase in processing time. However, the reverse did not hold, which parallels the finding in social psychology that men are penalized more for gender-nonconforming behavior. This suggests that language processing is not only affected by the strength of stereotype contexts; the associated disapproval of violating these gender stereotypes affects language processing, too.http://europepmc.org/articles/PMC6193704?pdf=render
collection DOAJ
language English
format Article
sources DOAJ
author Theresa Redl
Anita Eerland
Ted J M Sanders
spellingShingle Theresa Redl
Anita Eerland
Ted J M Sanders
The processing of the Dutch masculine generic zijn 'his' across stereotype contexts: An eye-tracking study.
PLoS ONE
author_facet Theresa Redl
Anita Eerland
Ted J M Sanders
author_sort Theresa Redl
title The processing of the Dutch masculine generic zijn 'his' across stereotype contexts: An eye-tracking study.
title_short The processing of the Dutch masculine generic zijn 'his' across stereotype contexts: An eye-tracking study.
title_full The processing of the Dutch masculine generic zijn 'his' across stereotype contexts: An eye-tracking study.
title_fullStr The processing of the Dutch masculine generic zijn 'his' across stereotype contexts: An eye-tracking study.
title_full_unstemmed The processing of the Dutch masculine generic zijn 'his' across stereotype contexts: An eye-tracking study.
title_sort processing of the dutch masculine generic zijn 'his' across stereotype contexts: an eye-tracking study.
publisher Public Library of Science (PLoS)
series PLoS ONE
issn 1932-6203
publishDate 2018-01-01
description Language users often infer a person's gender when it is not explicitly mentioned. This information is included in the mental model of the described situation, giving rise to expectations regarding the continuation of the discourse. Such gender inferences can be based on two types of information: gender stereotypes (e.g., nurses are female) and masculine generics, which are grammatically masculine word forms that are used to refer to all genders in certain contexts (e.g., To each his own). In this eye-tracking experiment (N = 82), which is the first to systematically investigate the online processing of masculine generic pronouns, we tested whether the frequently used Dutch masculine generic zijn 'his' leads to a male bias. In addition, we tested the effect of context by introducing male, female, and neutral stereotypes. We found no evidence for the hypothesis that the generically-intended masculine pronoun zijn 'his' results in a male bias. However, we found an effect of stereotype context. After introducing a female stereotype, reading about a man led to an increase in processing time. However, the reverse did not hold, which parallels the finding in social psychology that men are penalized more for gender-nonconforming behavior. This suggests that language processing is not only affected by the strength of stereotype contexts; the associated disapproval of violating these gender stereotypes affects language processing, too.
url http://europepmc.org/articles/PMC6193704?pdf=render
work_keys_str_mv AT theresaredl theprocessingofthedutchmasculinegenericzijnhisacrossstereotypecontextsaneyetrackingstudy
AT anitaeerland theprocessingofthedutchmasculinegenericzijnhisacrossstereotypecontextsaneyetrackingstudy
AT tedjmsanders theprocessingofthedutchmasculinegenericzijnhisacrossstereotypecontextsaneyetrackingstudy
AT theresaredl processingofthedutchmasculinegenericzijnhisacrossstereotypecontextsaneyetrackingstudy
AT anitaeerland processingofthedutchmasculinegenericzijnhisacrossstereotypecontextsaneyetrackingstudy
AT tedjmsanders processingofthedutchmasculinegenericzijnhisacrossstereotypecontextsaneyetrackingstudy
_version_ 1724992420605591552