Strength or Nausea? Children’s Reasoning About the Health Consequences of Food Consumption

Children’s reasoning on food properties and health relationships can contribute to healthier food choices. Food properties can either be positive (“gives strength”) or negative (“gives nausea”). One of the main challenges in public health is to foster children’s dietary variety, which contributes to...

Full description

Bibliographic Details
Main Authors: Damien Foinant, Jérémie Lafraire, Jean-Pierre Thibaut
Format: Article
Language:English
Published: Frontiers Media S.A. 2021-04-01
Series:Frontiers in Psychology
Subjects:
Online Access:https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fpsyg.2021.651889/full
id doaj-7a9e350150fa44fe803a38b21af904cc
record_format Article
spelling doaj-7a9e350150fa44fe803a38b21af904cc2021-04-08T13:34:59ZengFrontiers Media S.A.Frontiers in Psychology1664-10782021-04-011210.3389/fpsyg.2021.651889651889Strength or Nausea? Children’s Reasoning About the Health Consequences of Food ConsumptionDamien Foinant0Damien Foinant1Jérémie Lafraire2Jean-Pierre Thibaut3LEAD – Centre National de la Recherche Scientifique UMR-5022, Université Bourgogne Franche Comté, Dijon, FranceInstitut Paul Bocuse Research Center, Ecully, FranceInstitut Paul Bocuse Research Center, Ecully, FranceLEAD – Centre National de la Recherche Scientifique UMR-5022, Université Bourgogne Franche Comté, Dijon, FranceChildren’s reasoning on food properties and health relationships can contribute to healthier food choices. Food properties can either be positive (“gives strength”) or negative (“gives nausea”). One of the main challenges in public health is to foster children’s dietary variety, which contributes to a normal and healthy development. To face this challenge, it is essential to investigate how children generalize these positive and negative properties to other foods, including familiar and unfamiliar ones. In the present experiment, we hypothesized that children might rely on cues of food processing (e.g., signs of human intervention such as slicing) to convey information about item edibility. Furthermore, capitalizing on previous results showing that food rejections (i.e., food neophobia and picky eating) are a significant source of inter-individual variability to children’s inferences in the food domain, we followed an individual approach. We expected that children would generalize the positive properties to familiar foods and, in contrast, that they would generalize more often the negative properties to unfamiliar foods. However, we expected that children would generalize more positive and less negative properties to unfamiliar sliced foods than to whole unfamiliar foods. Finally, we expected that children displaying higher levels of food rejections would generalize more negative properties than children displaying lower levels of food rejections. One-hundred and twenty-six children, aged 3–6 years, performed an induction task in which they had to generalize positive or negative health-related properties to familiar or unfamiliar foods, whole or sliced. We measured children’s probability of generalization for positive and negative properties. The children’s food rejection score was assessed on a standardized scale. Results indicated that children evaluated positively familiar foods (regardless of processing), whereas they tend to view unfamiliar food negatively. In contrast, children were at chance for processed unfamiliar foods. Furthermore, children displaying higher levels of food rejections were more likely to generalize the negative properties to all kinds of foods than children displaying lower levels of food rejections. These findings entitle us to hypothesize that knowledge-based food education programs should take into account the valence of the properties taught to children, as well as the state of processing of the food presented. Furthermore, one should take children’s interindividual differences into account because they influence how the knowledge gained through these programs may be generalized.https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fpsyg.2021.651889/fullfood familiarityfood processingfood rejectioncognitioninductive reasoningneophobia
collection DOAJ
language English
format Article
sources DOAJ
author Damien Foinant
Damien Foinant
Jérémie Lafraire
Jean-Pierre Thibaut
spellingShingle Damien Foinant
Damien Foinant
Jérémie Lafraire
Jean-Pierre Thibaut
Strength or Nausea? Children’s Reasoning About the Health Consequences of Food Consumption
Frontiers in Psychology
food familiarity
food processing
food rejection
cognition
inductive reasoning
neophobia
author_facet Damien Foinant
Damien Foinant
Jérémie Lafraire
Jean-Pierre Thibaut
author_sort Damien Foinant
title Strength or Nausea? Children’s Reasoning About the Health Consequences of Food Consumption
title_short Strength or Nausea? Children’s Reasoning About the Health Consequences of Food Consumption
title_full Strength or Nausea? Children’s Reasoning About the Health Consequences of Food Consumption
title_fullStr Strength or Nausea? Children’s Reasoning About the Health Consequences of Food Consumption
title_full_unstemmed Strength or Nausea? Children’s Reasoning About the Health Consequences of Food Consumption
title_sort strength or nausea? children’s reasoning about the health consequences of food consumption
publisher Frontiers Media S.A.
series Frontiers in Psychology
issn 1664-1078
publishDate 2021-04-01
description Children’s reasoning on food properties and health relationships can contribute to healthier food choices. Food properties can either be positive (“gives strength”) or negative (“gives nausea”). One of the main challenges in public health is to foster children’s dietary variety, which contributes to a normal and healthy development. To face this challenge, it is essential to investigate how children generalize these positive and negative properties to other foods, including familiar and unfamiliar ones. In the present experiment, we hypothesized that children might rely on cues of food processing (e.g., signs of human intervention such as slicing) to convey information about item edibility. Furthermore, capitalizing on previous results showing that food rejections (i.e., food neophobia and picky eating) are a significant source of inter-individual variability to children’s inferences in the food domain, we followed an individual approach. We expected that children would generalize the positive properties to familiar foods and, in contrast, that they would generalize more often the negative properties to unfamiliar foods. However, we expected that children would generalize more positive and less negative properties to unfamiliar sliced foods than to whole unfamiliar foods. Finally, we expected that children displaying higher levels of food rejections would generalize more negative properties than children displaying lower levels of food rejections. One-hundred and twenty-six children, aged 3–6 years, performed an induction task in which they had to generalize positive or negative health-related properties to familiar or unfamiliar foods, whole or sliced. We measured children’s probability of generalization for positive and negative properties. The children’s food rejection score was assessed on a standardized scale. Results indicated that children evaluated positively familiar foods (regardless of processing), whereas they tend to view unfamiliar food negatively. In contrast, children were at chance for processed unfamiliar foods. Furthermore, children displaying higher levels of food rejections were more likely to generalize the negative properties to all kinds of foods than children displaying lower levels of food rejections. These findings entitle us to hypothesize that knowledge-based food education programs should take into account the valence of the properties taught to children, as well as the state of processing of the food presented. Furthermore, one should take children’s interindividual differences into account because they influence how the knowledge gained through these programs may be generalized.
topic food familiarity
food processing
food rejection
cognition
inductive reasoning
neophobia
url https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fpsyg.2021.651889/full
work_keys_str_mv AT damienfoinant strengthornauseachildrensreasoningaboutthehealthconsequencesoffoodconsumption
AT damienfoinant strengthornauseachildrensreasoningaboutthehealthconsequencesoffoodconsumption
AT jeremielafraire strengthornauseachildrensreasoningaboutthehealthconsequencesoffoodconsumption
AT jeanpierrethibaut strengthornauseachildrensreasoningaboutthehealthconsequencesoffoodconsumption
_version_ 1721535057510793216