Children's Exposure to Irony in the First Four Years of Their Life: What We Learn About the Use of Ironic Comments by Mothers from the Analysis of the Providence Corpus of Childes

There has been little research conducted on the use of figurative language in parents' input provided by caregivers in child-directed speech during the first four years of the child's life. The aim of the described study was to check (a) how often ironic comments are present in child-direc...

Full description

Bibliographic Details
Main Author: Banasik-Jemielniak Natalia
Format: Article
Language:English
Published: Sciendo 2019-01-01
Series:Psychology of Language and Communication
Subjects:
Online Access:https://doi.org/10.2478/plc-2019-0001
id doaj-6e20ca1c4cb746cc8967436c1337bc29
record_format Article
spelling doaj-6e20ca1c4cb746cc8967436c1337bc292021-09-05T14:01:05ZengSciendoPsychology of Language and Communication2083-85062019-01-0123111310.2478/plc-2019-0001plc-2019-0001Children's Exposure to Irony in the First Four Years of Their Life: What We Learn About the Use of Ironic Comments by Mothers from the Analysis of the Providence Corpus of ChildesBanasik-Jemielniak Natalia0Maria Grzegorzewska University, Poland University of Warsaw, PolandThere has been little research conducted on the use of figurative language in parents' input provided by caregivers in child-directed speech during the first four years of the child's life. The aim of the described study was to check (a) how often ironic comments are present in child-directed speech when the interaction takes place between a mother and a child aged 4 and below and (b) what types of ironic comments children of this age are exposed to. In order to answer these questions, ironic utterances were identified in the videos of 50 hours of recordings that included mother-child interactions of five children aged 2;10‒3;05, available through the CHILDES ‒ Providence Data (Demuth, Culbertson, & Alter, 2006; MacWhinney, 2007). The extracts were then assessed by competent judges to make sure the identified instances met the criteria for verbal irony (Dynel, 2014). Results suggest that irony is present in the mother's language used while interacting with her child, with a significant number of comments where the child seems not to be the actual addressee of the message, but rather the overhearer. The ironic utterances identified during the interactions included mostly references to the child's behavior or being overwhelmed. The most common ironic markers present in these utterances were rhetorical questions and hyperboles.https://doi.org/10.2478/plc-2019-0001ironychild-directed speechfigurative language
collection DOAJ
language English
format Article
sources DOAJ
author Banasik-Jemielniak Natalia
spellingShingle Banasik-Jemielniak Natalia
Children's Exposure to Irony in the First Four Years of Their Life: What We Learn About the Use of Ironic Comments by Mothers from the Analysis of the Providence Corpus of Childes
Psychology of Language and Communication
irony
child-directed speech
figurative language
author_facet Banasik-Jemielniak Natalia
author_sort Banasik-Jemielniak Natalia
title Children's Exposure to Irony in the First Four Years of Their Life: What We Learn About the Use of Ironic Comments by Mothers from the Analysis of the Providence Corpus of Childes
title_short Children's Exposure to Irony in the First Four Years of Their Life: What We Learn About the Use of Ironic Comments by Mothers from the Analysis of the Providence Corpus of Childes
title_full Children's Exposure to Irony in the First Four Years of Their Life: What We Learn About the Use of Ironic Comments by Mothers from the Analysis of the Providence Corpus of Childes
title_fullStr Children's Exposure to Irony in the First Four Years of Their Life: What We Learn About the Use of Ironic Comments by Mothers from the Analysis of the Providence Corpus of Childes
title_full_unstemmed Children's Exposure to Irony in the First Four Years of Their Life: What We Learn About the Use of Ironic Comments by Mothers from the Analysis of the Providence Corpus of Childes
title_sort children's exposure to irony in the first four years of their life: what we learn about the use of ironic comments by mothers from the analysis of the providence corpus of childes
publisher Sciendo
series Psychology of Language and Communication
issn 2083-8506
publishDate 2019-01-01
description There has been little research conducted on the use of figurative language in parents' input provided by caregivers in child-directed speech during the first four years of the child's life. The aim of the described study was to check (a) how often ironic comments are present in child-directed speech when the interaction takes place between a mother and a child aged 4 and below and (b) what types of ironic comments children of this age are exposed to. In order to answer these questions, ironic utterances were identified in the videos of 50 hours of recordings that included mother-child interactions of five children aged 2;10‒3;05, available through the CHILDES ‒ Providence Data (Demuth, Culbertson, & Alter, 2006; MacWhinney, 2007). The extracts were then assessed by competent judges to make sure the identified instances met the criteria for verbal irony (Dynel, 2014). Results suggest that irony is present in the mother's language used while interacting with her child, with a significant number of comments where the child seems not to be the actual addressee of the message, but rather the overhearer. The ironic utterances identified during the interactions included mostly references to the child's behavior or being overwhelmed. The most common ironic markers present in these utterances were rhetorical questions and hyperboles.
topic irony
child-directed speech
figurative language
url https://doi.org/10.2478/plc-2019-0001
work_keys_str_mv AT banasikjemielniaknatalia childrensexposuretoironyinthefirstfouryearsoftheirlifewhatwelearnabouttheuseofironiccommentsbymothersfromtheanalysisoftheprovidencecorpusofchildes
_version_ 1717810821788073984