Span–conditioned allomorphy and late linearization: Evidence from the Classical Greek perfect

Data from the perfect in Classical Greek provide empirical evidence for inwardly– and outwardly–sensitive span–conditioned allomorphy and indicate the need for a post–Vocabulary Insertion linearization process. The data also support the extremely late computation of the phonology of reduplicants. Pe...

Full description

Bibliographic Details
Main Author: Sylvia L. R. Schreiner
Format: Article
Language:English
Published: Open Library of Humanities 2021-05-01
Series:Glossa
Subjects:
Online Access:https://www.glossa-journal.org/articles/1400
id doaj-6bc16f5d207d4c79b4814f1a3db5f8b9
record_format Article
spelling doaj-6bc16f5d207d4c79b4814f1a3db5f8b92021-09-02T21:39:18ZengOpen Library of HumanitiesGlossa2397-18352021-05-016110.5334/gjgl.1400648Span–conditioned allomorphy and late linearization: Evidence from the Classical Greek perfectSylvia L. R. Schreiner0George Mason UniversityData from the perfect in Classical Greek provide empirical evidence for inwardly– and outwardly–sensitive span–conditioned allomorphy and indicate the need for a post–Vocabulary Insertion linearization process. The data also support the extremely late computation of the phonology of reduplicants. Perfect aspect in Classical Greek is realized via three distinct exponents: a reduplicative prefix, a suffix -/k/ (for some verbs) or stem allomorphy (for others), and a dedicated set of agreement suffixes. I argue that this case of Multiple Exponence results from one direct exponent of the Aspect[perfect] head and two cases of allomorphy at other nodes conditioned by spans that include the Aspect[perfect] head. The reduplicant is a Vocabulary Item (RED) that instantiates Aspect. Its phonology is determined after both Vocabulary Insertion and linearization. The -/k/ suffix is an outwardly–sensitive allomorph of Voice[active] conditioned by the span ⟨Aspect, Tense⟩, and perfect stem allomorphy in verbs that show it is conditioned by ⟨Voice, Aspect, Tense⟩. The agreement suffixes are inwardly–sensitive allomorphs conditioned by the span ⟨Voice, Aspect, Tense, Mood⟩. When taken together, the data indicate that Vocabulary Insertion must proceed cyclically, and that linearization must happen very late – after Vocabulary Insertion – since the realizations of both Voice[active] and AGR are conditioned by spans of hierarchically adjacent, rather than surface–contiguous, heads. The Greek data are essential for our understanding of the post–syntactic order of operations.https://www.glossa-journal.org/articles/1400span–conditioned allomorphylinearizationreduplicationclassical greekdistributed morphology
collection DOAJ
language English
format Article
sources DOAJ
author Sylvia L. R. Schreiner
spellingShingle Sylvia L. R. Schreiner
Span–conditioned allomorphy and late linearization: Evidence from the Classical Greek perfect
Glossa
span–conditioned allomorphy
linearization
reduplication
classical greek
distributed morphology
author_facet Sylvia L. R. Schreiner
author_sort Sylvia L. R. Schreiner
title Span–conditioned allomorphy and late linearization: Evidence from the Classical Greek perfect
title_short Span–conditioned allomorphy and late linearization: Evidence from the Classical Greek perfect
title_full Span–conditioned allomorphy and late linearization: Evidence from the Classical Greek perfect
title_fullStr Span–conditioned allomorphy and late linearization: Evidence from the Classical Greek perfect
title_full_unstemmed Span–conditioned allomorphy and late linearization: Evidence from the Classical Greek perfect
title_sort span–conditioned allomorphy and late linearization: evidence from the classical greek perfect
publisher Open Library of Humanities
series Glossa
issn 2397-1835
publishDate 2021-05-01
description Data from the perfect in Classical Greek provide empirical evidence for inwardly– and outwardly–sensitive span–conditioned allomorphy and indicate the need for a post–Vocabulary Insertion linearization process. The data also support the extremely late computation of the phonology of reduplicants. Perfect aspect in Classical Greek is realized via three distinct exponents: a reduplicative prefix, a suffix -/k/ (for some verbs) or stem allomorphy (for others), and a dedicated set of agreement suffixes. I argue that this case of Multiple Exponence results from one direct exponent of the Aspect[perfect] head and two cases of allomorphy at other nodes conditioned by spans that include the Aspect[perfect] head. The reduplicant is a Vocabulary Item (RED) that instantiates Aspect. Its phonology is determined after both Vocabulary Insertion and linearization. The -/k/ suffix is an outwardly–sensitive allomorph of Voice[active] conditioned by the span ⟨Aspect, Tense⟩, and perfect stem allomorphy in verbs that show it is conditioned by ⟨Voice, Aspect, Tense⟩. The agreement suffixes are inwardly–sensitive allomorphs conditioned by the span ⟨Voice, Aspect, Tense, Mood⟩. When taken together, the data indicate that Vocabulary Insertion must proceed cyclically, and that linearization must happen very late – after Vocabulary Insertion – since the realizations of both Voice[active] and AGR are conditioned by spans of hierarchically adjacent, rather than surface–contiguous, heads. The Greek data are essential for our understanding of the post–syntactic order of operations.
topic span–conditioned allomorphy
linearization
reduplication
classical greek
distributed morphology
url https://www.glossa-journal.org/articles/1400
work_keys_str_mv AT sylvialrschreiner spanconditionedallomorphyandlatelinearizationevidencefromtheclassicalgreekperfect
_version_ 1717819632899850240