Habituation to feedback delay restores degraded visuomotor adaptation by altering both sensory prediction error and the sensitivity of the adaptation to the error

Sensory prediction error, which is the difference between actual and predicted sensory consequences, is a driving force of motor learning. Thus, appropriate temporal associations between the actual sensory feedback signals and motor commands for predicting sensory consequences are crucial for the br...

Full description

Bibliographic Details
Main Authors: Takuya eHonda, Masaya eHirashima, Daichi eNozaki
Format: Article
Language:English
Published: Frontiers Media S.A. 2012-12-01
Series:Frontiers in Psychology
Subjects:
Online Access:http://journal.frontiersin.org/Journal/10.3389/fpsyg.2012.00540/full
id doaj-4b0e16471d234c55a3072b2728993098
record_format Article
spelling doaj-4b0e16471d234c55a3072b27289930982020-11-24T22:02:44ZengFrontiers Media S.A.Frontiers in Psychology1664-10782012-12-01310.3389/fpsyg.2012.0054034974Habituation to feedback delay restores degraded visuomotor adaptation by altering both sensory prediction error and the sensitivity of the adaptation to the errorTakuya eHonda0Takuya eHonda1Takuya eHonda2Masaya eHirashima3Daichi eNozaki4The University of TokyoThe Japanese Society for the Promotion of ScienceAdvanced Telecommunications Research Institute InternationalThe University of TokyoThe University of TokyoSensory prediction error, which is the difference between actual and predicted sensory consequences, is a driving force of motor learning. Thus, appropriate temporal associations between the actual sensory feedback signals and motor commands for predicting sensory consequences are crucial for the brain to calculate the sensory prediction error accurately. Indeed, it has been shown that artificially introduced delays in visual feedback degrade motor learning. However, our previous study has showed that degraded adaptation is alleviated by prior habituation to the delay. Here, we investigate how the motor learning system accomplishes this alleviation. After the subjects habituated reaching movements in either 0-ms or 200-ms-delayed cursor, visual rotation of 10° was imposed to the cursor with varying delay (0, 100, 200, or 300 ms) with each delay imposed in at least 1 out of 5-6 trials. Then, the aftereffect in the next trial was quantified to evaluate the adaptation response. After habituation to the 0-ms delayed cursor, the adaptation response was maximal when the visual feedback of the perturbation was provided with 0-ms delay and gradually decreased as the delay increased. On the other hand, habituation to the 200-ms delayed cursor alleviated the degraded adaptation response to the visual perturbation imposed during the 200-ms and longer delay (300-ms). However, habituation did not affect the adaptation response to the visual perturbation imposed during delays (0- and 100-ms delay) shorter than the habituated delay (200-ms). These results may be explained by assuming that habituation to the delayed feedback not only shifts the position of the hand predicted by motor command toward the delayed cursor positions, but also increases the degree to which the brain uses a certain amount of sensory prediction error to correct a motor command.http://journal.frontiersin.org/Journal/10.3389/fpsyg.2012.00540/fullfeedback delayvisuomotor adaptationforward modeldelay adaptationsensorimotor association
collection DOAJ
language English
format Article
sources DOAJ
author Takuya eHonda
Takuya eHonda
Takuya eHonda
Masaya eHirashima
Daichi eNozaki
spellingShingle Takuya eHonda
Takuya eHonda
Takuya eHonda
Masaya eHirashima
Daichi eNozaki
Habituation to feedback delay restores degraded visuomotor adaptation by altering both sensory prediction error and the sensitivity of the adaptation to the error
Frontiers in Psychology
feedback delay
visuomotor adaptation
forward model
delay adaptation
sensorimotor association
author_facet Takuya eHonda
Takuya eHonda
Takuya eHonda
Masaya eHirashima
Daichi eNozaki
author_sort Takuya eHonda
title Habituation to feedback delay restores degraded visuomotor adaptation by altering both sensory prediction error and the sensitivity of the adaptation to the error
title_short Habituation to feedback delay restores degraded visuomotor adaptation by altering both sensory prediction error and the sensitivity of the adaptation to the error
title_full Habituation to feedback delay restores degraded visuomotor adaptation by altering both sensory prediction error and the sensitivity of the adaptation to the error
title_fullStr Habituation to feedback delay restores degraded visuomotor adaptation by altering both sensory prediction error and the sensitivity of the adaptation to the error
title_full_unstemmed Habituation to feedback delay restores degraded visuomotor adaptation by altering both sensory prediction error and the sensitivity of the adaptation to the error
title_sort habituation to feedback delay restores degraded visuomotor adaptation by altering both sensory prediction error and the sensitivity of the adaptation to the error
publisher Frontiers Media S.A.
series Frontiers in Psychology
issn 1664-1078
publishDate 2012-12-01
description Sensory prediction error, which is the difference between actual and predicted sensory consequences, is a driving force of motor learning. Thus, appropriate temporal associations between the actual sensory feedback signals and motor commands for predicting sensory consequences are crucial for the brain to calculate the sensory prediction error accurately. Indeed, it has been shown that artificially introduced delays in visual feedback degrade motor learning. However, our previous study has showed that degraded adaptation is alleviated by prior habituation to the delay. Here, we investigate how the motor learning system accomplishes this alleviation. After the subjects habituated reaching movements in either 0-ms or 200-ms-delayed cursor, visual rotation of 10° was imposed to the cursor with varying delay (0, 100, 200, or 300 ms) with each delay imposed in at least 1 out of 5-6 trials. Then, the aftereffect in the next trial was quantified to evaluate the adaptation response. After habituation to the 0-ms delayed cursor, the adaptation response was maximal when the visual feedback of the perturbation was provided with 0-ms delay and gradually decreased as the delay increased. On the other hand, habituation to the 200-ms delayed cursor alleviated the degraded adaptation response to the visual perturbation imposed during the 200-ms and longer delay (300-ms). However, habituation did not affect the adaptation response to the visual perturbation imposed during delays (0- and 100-ms delay) shorter than the habituated delay (200-ms). These results may be explained by assuming that habituation to the delayed feedback not only shifts the position of the hand predicted by motor command toward the delayed cursor positions, but also increases the degree to which the brain uses a certain amount of sensory prediction error to correct a motor command.
topic feedback delay
visuomotor adaptation
forward model
delay adaptation
sensorimotor association
url http://journal.frontiersin.org/Journal/10.3389/fpsyg.2012.00540/full
work_keys_str_mv AT takuyaehonda habituationtofeedbackdelayrestoresdegradedvisuomotoradaptationbyalteringbothsensorypredictionerrorandthesensitivityoftheadaptationtotheerror
AT takuyaehonda habituationtofeedbackdelayrestoresdegradedvisuomotoradaptationbyalteringbothsensorypredictionerrorandthesensitivityoftheadaptationtotheerror
AT takuyaehonda habituationtofeedbackdelayrestoresdegradedvisuomotoradaptationbyalteringbothsensorypredictionerrorandthesensitivityoftheadaptationtotheerror
AT masayaehirashima habituationtofeedbackdelayrestoresdegradedvisuomotoradaptationbyalteringbothsensorypredictionerrorandthesensitivityoftheadaptationtotheerror
AT daichienozaki habituationtofeedbackdelayrestoresdegradedvisuomotoradaptationbyalteringbothsensorypredictionerrorandthesensitivityoftheadaptationtotheerror
_version_ 1725834235679342592