Monolingual and Bilingual Infants’ Ability to Use Non-native Tone for Word Learning Deteriorates by the Second Year After Birth

Previous studies reported a non-native word learning advantage for bilingual infants at around 18 months. We investigated developmental changes in infant interpretation of sounds that aid in object mapping. Dutch monolingual and bilingual (exposed to Dutch and a second non-tone-language) infants’ wo...

Full description

Bibliographic Details
Main Authors: Liquan Liu, René Kager
Format: Article
Language:English
Published: Frontiers Media S.A. 2018-03-01
Series:Frontiers in Psychology
Subjects:
Online Access:http://journal.frontiersin.org/article/10.3389/fpsyg.2018.00117/full
id doaj-2d7f1ad1132a44b8b73cd1fabce7c0f2
record_format Article
spelling doaj-2d7f1ad1132a44b8b73cd1fabce7c0f22020-11-25T00:00:27ZengFrontiers Media S.A.Frontiers in Psychology1664-10782018-03-01910.3389/fpsyg.2018.00117290042Monolingual and Bilingual Infants’ Ability to Use Non-native Tone for Word Learning Deteriorates by the Second Year After BirthLiquan Liu0Liquan Liu1Liquan Liu2Liquan Liu3René Kager4School of Social Sciences and Psychology, Western Sydney University, Sydney, NSW, AustraliaUtrecht Institute of Linguistics-OTS, Utrecht University, Utrecht, NetherlandsMARCS Institute for Brain, Behaviour & Development, Western Sydney University, Sydney, NSW, AustraliaCentre of Excellence for the Dynamics of Language, Australian Research Council, Canberra, ACT, AustraliaUtrecht Institute of Linguistics-OTS, Utrecht University, Utrecht, NetherlandsPrevious studies reported a non-native word learning advantage for bilingual infants at around 18 months. We investigated developmental changes in infant interpretation of sounds that aid in object mapping. Dutch monolingual and bilingual (exposed to Dutch and a second non-tone-language) infants’ word learning ability was examined on two novel label–object pairings using syllables differing in Mandarin tones as labels (flat vs. falling). Infants aged 14–15 months, regardless of language backgrounds, were sensitive to violations in the label–objects pairings when lexical tones were switched compared to when they were the same as habituated. Conversely at 17–18 months, neither monolingual nor bilingual infants demonstrated learning. Linking with existing literature, infants’ ability to associate non-native tones with meanings may be related to tonal acoustic properties and/or perceptual assimilation to native prosodic categories. These findings provide new insights into the relation between infant tone perception, learning, and interpretative narrowing from a developmental perspective.http://journal.frontiersin.org/article/10.3389/fpsyg.2018.00117/fulllabel–object mappinglexical tonebilingualisminterpretive narrowingperceptual assimilation
collection DOAJ
language English
format Article
sources DOAJ
author Liquan Liu
Liquan Liu
Liquan Liu
Liquan Liu
René Kager
spellingShingle Liquan Liu
Liquan Liu
Liquan Liu
Liquan Liu
René Kager
Monolingual and Bilingual Infants’ Ability to Use Non-native Tone for Word Learning Deteriorates by the Second Year After Birth
Frontiers in Psychology
label–object mapping
lexical tone
bilingualism
interpretive narrowing
perceptual assimilation
author_facet Liquan Liu
Liquan Liu
Liquan Liu
Liquan Liu
René Kager
author_sort Liquan Liu
title Monolingual and Bilingual Infants’ Ability to Use Non-native Tone for Word Learning Deteriorates by the Second Year After Birth
title_short Monolingual and Bilingual Infants’ Ability to Use Non-native Tone for Word Learning Deteriorates by the Second Year After Birth
title_full Monolingual and Bilingual Infants’ Ability to Use Non-native Tone for Word Learning Deteriorates by the Second Year After Birth
title_fullStr Monolingual and Bilingual Infants’ Ability to Use Non-native Tone for Word Learning Deteriorates by the Second Year After Birth
title_full_unstemmed Monolingual and Bilingual Infants’ Ability to Use Non-native Tone for Word Learning Deteriorates by the Second Year After Birth
title_sort monolingual and bilingual infants’ ability to use non-native tone for word learning deteriorates by the second year after birth
publisher Frontiers Media S.A.
series Frontiers in Psychology
issn 1664-1078
publishDate 2018-03-01
description Previous studies reported a non-native word learning advantage for bilingual infants at around 18 months. We investigated developmental changes in infant interpretation of sounds that aid in object mapping. Dutch monolingual and bilingual (exposed to Dutch and a second non-tone-language) infants’ word learning ability was examined on two novel label–object pairings using syllables differing in Mandarin tones as labels (flat vs. falling). Infants aged 14–15 months, regardless of language backgrounds, were sensitive to violations in the label–objects pairings when lexical tones were switched compared to when they were the same as habituated. Conversely at 17–18 months, neither monolingual nor bilingual infants demonstrated learning. Linking with existing literature, infants’ ability to associate non-native tones with meanings may be related to tonal acoustic properties and/or perceptual assimilation to native prosodic categories. These findings provide new insights into the relation between infant tone perception, learning, and interpretative narrowing from a developmental perspective.
topic label–object mapping
lexical tone
bilingualism
interpretive narrowing
perceptual assimilation
url http://journal.frontiersin.org/article/10.3389/fpsyg.2018.00117/full
work_keys_str_mv AT liquanliu monolingualandbilingualinfantsabilitytousenonnativetoneforwordlearningdeterioratesbythesecondyearafterbirth
AT liquanliu monolingualandbilingualinfantsabilitytousenonnativetoneforwordlearningdeterioratesbythesecondyearafterbirth
AT liquanliu monolingualandbilingualinfantsabilitytousenonnativetoneforwordlearningdeterioratesbythesecondyearafterbirth
AT liquanliu monolingualandbilingualinfantsabilitytousenonnativetoneforwordlearningdeterioratesbythesecondyearafterbirth
AT renekager monolingualandbilingualinfantsabilitytousenonnativetoneforwordlearningdeterioratesbythesecondyearafterbirth
_version_ 1725445083729231872