Vigilant conservatism in evaluating communicated information.

In the absence of other information, people put more weight on their own opinion than on the opinion of others: they are conservative. Several proximal mechanisms have been suggested to account for this finding. One of these mechanisms is that people cannot access reasons for other people's opi...

Full description

Bibliographic Details
Main Authors: Emmanuel Trouche, Petter Johansson, Lars Hall, Hugo Mercier
Format: Article
Language:English
Published: Public Library of Science (PLoS) 2018-01-01
Series:PLoS ONE
Online Access:http://europepmc.org/articles/PMC5762162?pdf=render
id doaj-1a22c22d81b74cd8863a5e5b52b21954
record_format Article
spelling doaj-1a22c22d81b74cd8863a5e5b52b219542020-11-24T22:05:31ZengPublic Library of Science (PLoS)PLoS ONE1932-62032018-01-01131e018882510.1371/journal.pone.0188825Vigilant conservatism in evaluating communicated information.Emmanuel TrouchePetter JohanssonLars HallHugo MercierIn the absence of other information, people put more weight on their own opinion than on the opinion of others: they are conservative. Several proximal mechanisms have been suggested to account for this finding. One of these mechanisms is that people cannot access reasons for other people's opinions, but they can access the reasons for their own opinions-whether they are the actual reasons that led them to hold the opinions (rational access to reasons), or post-hoc constructions (biased access to reasons). In four experiments, participants were asked to provide an opinion, and then faced with another participant's opinion and asked if they wanted to revise their initial opinion. Some questions were manipulated so that the advice participants were receiving was in fact their own opinion, while what they thought was their own opinion was in fact not. In all experiments, the participants were consistently biased towards what they thought was their own opinion, showing that conservativeness cannot be explained by rational access to reasons, which should have favored the advice. One experiment revealed that conservativeness was not decreased under time pressure, suggesting that biased access to reasons is an unlikely explanation for conservativeness. The experiments also suggest that repetition plays a role in advice taking, with repeated opinions being granted more weight than non-fluent opinions. Our results are not consistent with any of the established proximal explanations for conservatism. Instead, we suggest an ultimate explanation-vigilant conservatism-that sees conservatism as adaptive since receivers should be wary of senders' interests, as they rarely perfectly converge with theirs.http://europepmc.org/articles/PMC5762162?pdf=render
collection DOAJ
language English
format Article
sources DOAJ
author Emmanuel Trouche
Petter Johansson
Lars Hall
Hugo Mercier
spellingShingle Emmanuel Trouche
Petter Johansson
Lars Hall
Hugo Mercier
Vigilant conservatism in evaluating communicated information.
PLoS ONE
author_facet Emmanuel Trouche
Petter Johansson
Lars Hall
Hugo Mercier
author_sort Emmanuel Trouche
title Vigilant conservatism in evaluating communicated information.
title_short Vigilant conservatism in evaluating communicated information.
title_full Vigilant conservatism in evaluating communicated information.
title_fullStr Vigilant conservatism in evaluating communicated information.
title_full_unstemmed Vigilant conservatism in evaluating communicated information.
title_sort vigilant conservatism in evaluating communicated information.
publisher Public Library of Science (PLoS)
series PLoS ONE
issn 1932-6203
publishDate 2018-01-01
description In the absence of other information, people put more weight on their own opinion than on the opinion of others: they are conservative. Several proximal mechanisms have been suggested to account for this finding. One of these mechanisms is that people cannot access reasons for other people's opinions, but they can access the reasons for their own opinions-whether they are the actual reasons that led them to hold the opinions (rational access to reasons), or post-hoc constructions (biased access to reasons). In four experiments, participants were asked to provide an opinion, and then faced with another participant's opinion and asked if they wanted to revise their initial opinion. Some questions were manipulated so that the advice participants were receiving was in fact their own opinion, while what they thought was their own opinion was in fact not. In all experiments, the participants were consistently biased towards what they thought was their own opinion, showing that conservativeness cannot be explained by rational access to reasons, which should have favored the advice. One experiment revealed that conservativeness was not decreased under time pressure, suggesting that biased access to reasons is an unlikely explanation for conservativeness. The experiments also suggest that repetition plays a role in advice taking, with repeated opinions being granted more weight than non-fluent opinions. Our results are not consistent with any of the established proximal explanations for conservatism. Instead, we suggest an ultimate explanation-vigilant conservatism-that sees conservatism as adaptive since receivers should be wary of senders' interests, as they rarely perfectly converge with theirs.
url http://europepmc.org/articles/PMC5762162?pdf=render
work_keys_str_mv AT emmanueltrouche vigilantconservatisminevaluatingcommunicatedinformation
AT petterjohansson vigilantconservatisminevaluatingcommunicatedinformation
AT larshall vigilantconservatisminevaluatingcommunicatedinformation
AT hugomercier vigilantconservatisminevaluatingcommunicatedinformation
_version_ 1725826081229897728