Effect of restricting bedtime mobile phone use on sleep, arousal, mood, and working memory: A randomized pilot trial.
BACKGROUND:This study aimed to assess the effects of restricting mobile phone use before bedtime on sleep, pre-sleep arousal, mood, and working memory. METHODS:Thirty-eight participants were randomized to either an intervention group (n = 19), where members were instructed to avoid using their mobil...
Main Authors: | , , , , |
---|---|
Format: | Article |
Language: | English |
Published: |
Public Library of Science (PLoS)
2020-01-01
|
Series: | PLoS ONE |
Online Access: | https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0228756 |
id |
doaj-1198ff0a00b64c9e872d9e552a126928 |
---|---|
record_format |
Article |
spelling |
doaj-1198ff0a00b64c9e872d9e552a1269282021-03-03T21:29:05ZengPublic Library of Science (PLoS)PLoS ONE1932-62032020-01-01152e022875610.1371/journal.pone.0228756Effect of restricting bedtime mobile phone use on sleep, arousal, mood, and working memory: A randomized pilot trial.Jing-Wen HeZhi-Hao TuLei XiaoTong SuYun-Xiang TangBACKGROUND:This study aimed to assess the effects of restricting mobile phone use before bedtime on sleep, pre-sleep arousal, mood, and working memory. METHODS:Thirty-eight participants were randomized to either an intervention group (n = 19), where members were instructed to avoid using their mobile phone 30 minutes before bedtime, or a control group (n = 19), where the participants were given no such instructions. Sleep habit, sleep quality, pre-sleep arousal and mood were measured using the sleep diary, the Pittsburgh sleep quality index, the Pre-sleep Arousal Scale and the Positive and Negative Affect Schedule respectively. Working memory was tested by using the 0-,1-,2-back task (n-back task). RESULTS:Restricting mobile phone use before bedtime for four weeks was effective in reducing sleep latency, increasing sleep duration, improving sleep quality, reducing pre-sleep arousal, and improving positive affect and working memory. CONCLUSIONS:Restricting mobile phone use close to bedtime reduced sleep latency and pre-sleep arousal and increased sleep duration and working memory. This simple change to moderate usage was recommended to individuals with sleep disturbances.https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0228756 |
collection |
DOAJ |
language |
English |
format |
Article |
sources |
DOAJ |
author |
Jing-Wen He Zhi-Hao Tu Lei Xiao Tong Su Yun-Xiang Tang |
spellingShingle |
Jing-Wen He Zhi-Hao Tu Lei Xiao Tong Su Yun-Xiang Tang Effect of restricting bedtime mobile phone use on sleep, arousal, mood, and working memory: A randomized pilot trial. PLoS ONE |
author_facet |
Jing-Wen He Zhi-Hao Tu Lei Xiao Tong Su Yun-Xiang Tang |
author_sort |
Jing-Wen He |
title |
Effect of restricting bedtime mobile phone use on sleep, arousal, mood, and working memory: A randomized pilot trial. |
title_short |
Effect of restricting bedtime mobile phone use on sleep, arousal, mood, and working memory: A randomized pilot trial. |
title_full |
Effect of restricting bedtime mobile phone use on sleep, arousal, mood, and working memory: A randomized pilot trial. |
title_fullStr |
Effect of restricting bedtime mobile phone use on sleep, arousal, mood, and working memory: A randomized pilot trial. |
title_full_unstemmed |
Effect of restricting bedtime mobile phone use on sleep, arousal, mood, and working memory: A randomized pilot trial. |
title_sort |
effect of restricting bedtime mobile phone use on sleep, arousal, mood, and working memory: a randomized pilot trial. |
publisher |
Public Library of Science (PLoS) |
series |
PLoS ONE |
issn |
1932-6203 |
publishDate |
2020-01-01 |
description |
BACKGROUND:This study aimed to assess the effects of restricting mobile phone use before bedtime on sleep, pre-sleep arousal, mood, and working memory. METHODS:Thirty-eight participants were randomized to either an intervention group (n = 19), where members were instructed to avoid using their mobile phone 30 minutes before bedtime, or a control group (n = 19), where the participants were given no such instructions. Sleep habit, sleep quality, pre-sleep arousal and mood were measured using the sleep diary, the Pittsburgh sleep quality index, the Pre-sleep Arousal Scale and the Positive and Negative Affect Schedule respectively. Working memory was tested by using the 0-,1-,2-back task (n-back task). RESULTS:Restricting mobile phone use before bedtime for four weeks was effective in reducing sleep latency, increasing sleep duration, improving sleep quality, reducing pre-sleep arousal, and improving positive affect and working memory. CONCLUSIONS:Restricting mobile phone use close to bedtime reduced sleep latency and pre-sleep arousal and increased sleep duration and working memory. This simple change to moderate usage was recommended to individuals with sleep disturbances. |
url |
https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0228756 |
work_keys_str_mv |
AT jingwenhe effectofrestrictingbedtimemobilephoneuseonsleeparousalmoodandworkingmemoryarandomizedpilottrial AT zhihaotu effectofrestrictingbedtimemobilephoneuseonsleeparousalmoodandworkingmemoryarandomizedpilottrial AT leixiao effectofrestrictingbedtimemobilephoneuseonsleeparousalmoodandworkingmemoryarandomizedpilottrial AT tongsu effectofrestrictingbedtimemobilephoneuseonsleeparousalmoodandworkingmemoryarandomizedpilottrial AT yunxiangtang effectofrestrictingbedtimemobilephoneuseonsleeparousalmoodandworkingmemoryarandomizedpilottrial |
_version_ |
1714816580629561344 |