Characterizing the mathematical problem-solving strategies of transitioning novice physics students

Much work has been done to characterize the reasoning of students as they solve mathematics-intensive problems and characterizing differences in expert and novice problem solving. In this work, we characterize the problem-solving strategies in a classroom setting of “transitioning novices,” students...

Full description

Bibliographic Details
Main Authors: Eric Burkholder, Lena Blackmon, Carl Wieman
Format: Article
Language:English
Published: American Physical Society 2020-11-01
Series:Physical Review Physics Education Research
Online Access:http://doi.org/10.1103/PhysRevPhysEducRes.16.020134
id doaj-0f207bac74a4429f9eba5cc9af596e56
record_format Article
spelling doaj-0f207bac74a4429f9eba5cc9af596e562020-11-25T04:00:24ZengAmerican Physical SocietyPhysical Review Physics Education Research2469-98962020-11-0116202013410.1103/PhysRevPhysEducRes.16.020134Characterizing the mathematical problem-solving strategies of transitioning novice physics studentsEric BurkholderLena BlackmonCarl WiemanMuch work has been done to characterize the reasoning of students as they solve mathematics-intensive problems and characterizing differences in expert and novice problem solving. In this work, we characterize the problem-solving strategies in a classroom setting of “transitioning novices,” students who have completed an introductory physics course and have learned some problem-solving strategies, but are far from expertlike in their reasoning. We find that students mostly use intermediate strategies that reflect an understanding of specific relationships between quantities, such as analyzing the units of an expression, to reason about mathematical expressions. Few students use more sophisticated strategies like checking limits, which require students to run mental simulations to predict how a system will behave as different physical variables are changed. The teaching of more advanced strategies like limit checking will require careful scaffolding of the cognitive complexity, as students generally do not succeed when simply told to check limits. This is supported by the findings of Lin and Singh [Phys. Rev. Phys. Educ. Res. 7, 020104 (2011)10.1103/PhysRevSTPER.7.020104] that careful scaffolding is needed to help students solve more complex problems. In this particular group, students were able to successfully analyze the dimensions of an expression and compute component forces and torques to check if their answer made sense. Our results show that there is a need to recognize and teach these intermediary strategies to enable more novice students to check their answers and encourage students to become more expertlike.http://doi.org/10.1103/PhysRevPhysEducRes.16.020134
collection DOAJ
language English
format Article
sources DOAJ
author Eric Burkholder
Lena Blackmon
Carl Wieman
spellingShingle Eric Burkholder
Lena Blackmon
Carl Wieman
Characterizing the mathematical problem-solving strategies of transitioning novice physics students
Physical Review Physics Education Research
author_facet Eric Burkholder
Lena Blackmon
Carl Wieman
author_sort Eric Burkholder
title Characterizing the mathematical problem-solving strategies of transitioning novice physics students
title_short Characterizing the mathematical problem-solving strategies of transitioning novice physics students
title_full Characterizing the mathematical problem-solving strategies of transitioning novice physics students
title_fullStr Characterizing the mathematical problem-solving strategies of transitioning novice physics students
title_full_unstemmed Characterizing the mathematical problem-solving strategies of transitioning novice physics students
title_sort characterizing the mathematical problem-solving strategies of transitioning novice physics students
publisher American Physical Society
series Physical Review Physics Education Research
issn 2469-9896
publishDate 2020-11-01
description Much work has been done to characterize the reasoning of students as they solve mathematics-intensive problems and characterizing differences in expert and novice problem solving. In this work, we characterize the problem-solving strategies in a classroom setting of “transitioning novices,” students who have completed an introductory physics course and have learned some problem-solving strategies, but are far from expertlike in their reasoning. We find that students mostly use intermediate strategies that reflect an understanding of specific relationships between quantities, such as analyzing the units of an expression, to reason about mathematical expressions. Few students use more sophisticated strategies like checking limits, which require students to run mental simulations to predict how a system will behave as different physical variables are changed. The teaching of more advanced strategies like limit checking will require careful scaffolding of the cognitive complexity, as students generally do not succeed when simply told to check limits. This is supported by the findings of Lin and Singh [Phys. Rev. Phys. Educ. Res. 7, 020104 (2011)10.1103/PhysRevSTPER.7.020104] that careful scaffolding is needed to help students solve more complex problems. In this particular group, students were able to successfully analyze the dimensions of an expression and compute component forces and torques to check if their answer made sense. Our results show that there is a need to recognize and teach these intermediary strategies to enable more novice students to check their answers and encourage students to become more expertlike.
url http://doi.org/10.1103/PhysRevPhysEducRes.16.020134
work_keys_str_mv AT ericburkholder characterizingthemathematicalproblemsolvingstrategiesoftransitioningnovicephysicsstudents
AT lenablackmon characterizingthemathematicalproblemsolvingstrategiesoftransitioningnovicephysicsstudents
AT carlwieman characterizingthemathematicalproblemsolvingstrategiesoftransitioningnovicephysicsstudents
_version_ 1724450853642829824