Case/agreement matching: Evidence for a cognitive bias
In an artificial language experiment, participants were taught two different artificial languages consisting of English content words and novel morphological marking. The first of the languages had matching alignment in both case and agreement, as attested in natural languages such as Basque, Belhar...
Main Authors: | , , |
---|---|
Format: | Article |
Language: | English |
Published: |
Open Library of Humanities
2018-08-01
|
Series: | Glossa |
Subjects: | |
Online Access: | https://www.glossa-journal.org/articles/413 |
id |
doaj-0c006306efee442882214d726c09e1d7 |
---|---|
record_format |
Article |
spelling |
doaj-0c006306efee442882214d726c09e1d72021-09-02T09:43:02ZengOpen Library of HumanitiesGlossa2397-18352018-08-013110.5334/gjgl.413257Case/agreement matching: Evidence for a cognitive biasMichelle Sheehan0Albertyna Paciorek1John Williams2Anglia Ruskin UniversityUniversity of WarsawUniversity of CambridgeIn an artificial language experiment, participants were taught two different artificial languages consisting of English content words and novel morphological marking. The first of the languages had matching alignment in both case and agreement, as attested in natural languages such as Basque, Belhare and Tsez. The other language combined accusative case alignment with ergative agreement alignment, a combination which is apparently unattested amongst natural languages. There was no significant difference between the languages in terms of the proportion of participants that showed awareness of the agreement pattern, nor in the ability of aware participants to recall case markers and inflections during training, or select the correct verb inflection in the generation post-test. However, amongst participants who remained unaware of the agreement pattern there was a significant difference in recall of verb inflections and case markers during the exposure phase task – recall was more accurate in the (attested) language with matching case and agreement alignment than the (nonattested) language in which case and agreement alignment were unmatched. We take this as evidence that there is a cognitive bias against the unattested non-matching alignment, reflected in implicit learning.https://www.glossa-journal.org/articles/413agreementcasealignmentartificial languageergativityimplicit learning |
collection |
DOAJ |
language |
English |
format |
Article |
sources |
DOAJ |
author |
Michelle Sheehan Albertyna Paciorek John Williams |
spellingShingle |
Michelle Sheehan Albertyna Paciorek John Williams Case/agreement matching: Evidence for a cognitive bias Glossa agreement case alignment artificial language ergativity implicit learning |
author_facet |
Michelle Sheehan Albertyna Paciorek John Williams |
author_sort |
Michelle Sheehan |
title |
Case/agreement matching: Evidence for a cognitive bias |
title_short |
Case/agreement matching: Evidence for a cognitive bias |
title_full |
Case/agreement matching: Evidence for a cognitive bias |
title_fullStr |
Case/agreement matching: Evidence for a cognitive bias |
title_full_unstemmed |
Case/agreement matching: Evidence for a cognitive bias |
title_sort |
case/agreement matching: evidence for a cognitive bias |
publisher |
Open Library of Humanities |
series |
Glossa |
issn |
2397-1835 |
publishDate |
2018-08-01 |
description |
In an artificial language experiment, participants were taught two different artificial languages consisting of English content words and novel morphological marking. The first of the languages had matching alignment in both case and agreement, as attested in natural languages such as Basque, Belhare and Tsez. The other language combined accusative case alignment with ergative agreement alignment, a combination which is apparently unattested amongst natural languages. There was no significant difference between the languages in terms of the proportion of participants that showed awareness of the agreement pattern, nor in the ability of aware participants to recall case markers and inflections during training, or select the correct verb inflection in the generation post-test. However, amongst participants who remained unaware of the agreement pattern there was a significant difference in recall of verb inflections and case markers during the exposure phase task – recall was more accurate in the (attested) language with matching case and agreement alignment than the (nonattested) language in which case and agreement alignment were unmatched. We take this as evidence that there is a cognitive bias against the unattested non-matching alignment, reflected in implicit learning. |
topic |
agreement case alignment artificial language ergativity implicit learning |
url |
https://www.glossa-journal.org/articles/413 |
work_keys_str_mv |
AT michellesheehan caseagreementmatchingevidenceforacognitivebias AT albertynapaciorek caseagreementmatchingevidenceforacognitivebias AT johnwilliams caseagreementmatchingevidenceforacognitivebias |
_version_ |
1721176853450850304 |